Jump to content

Angular gyrus

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Prelunate gyrus)
Angular gyrus
  Angular gyrus
Drawing of a cast to illustrate the relations of the brain to the skull. (Angular gyrus labeled at upper left, in yellow section.)
Details
Identifiers
Latingyrus angularis
NeuroNames109
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1376
TA98A14.1.09.124
TA25472
FMA61898
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
In this image, the angular gyrus is denoted by the double asterisk **

The angular gyrus is a region of the brain lying mainly in the posteroinferior region of the parietal lobe, occupying the posterior part of the inferior parietal lobule.[1] It represents the Brodmann area 39.[1]

Its significance is in transferring visual information to Wernicke's area, in order to make meaning out of visually perceived words.[2] It is also involved in a number of processes related to language, number processing and spatial cognition, memory retrieval, attention, and theory of mind.

Anatomy

[edit]

Connections

[edit]

Left and right angular gyri are connected by the dorsal splenium and isthmus of the corpus callosum.[3]

Connections To the Angular gyrus
Connected To The Via the
ipsilateral frontal and caudallateral prefrontal and inferior frontal regions superior longitudinal fasciculus.[4]
caudate inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus[5]
parahippocampal gyrus[6] and hippocampus[5] inferior longitudinal fasciculus
precuneus and superior frontal gyrus occipitofrontal fasciculus,[7]
supramarginal gyrus local arcuate[8]

Boundaries

[edit]

Function

[edit]

The angular gyrus is the part of the brain associated with complex language functions (i.e. reading, writing and interpretation of what is written). Lesion to this part of the brain shows symptoms of the Gerstmann syndrome: effects include finger agnosia, alexia (inability to read), acalculia (inability to use arithmetic operations), agraphia (inability to copy), and left-right confusion.

Language

[edit]

Norman Geschwind proposed that written word is translated to internal monologue via the angular gyrus.[citation needed]

V. S. Ramachandran, and Edward Hubbard published a paper in 2003 in which they hypothesized the angular gyrus to play a role in understanding metaphors. They stated:

There may be neurological disorders that disturb metaphor and synaesthesia. This has not been studied in detail but we have seen disturbances in the Bouba/Kiki effect (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a) as well as with proverbs in patients with angular gyrus lesions. It would be interesting to see whether they have deficits in other types of synaesthetic metaphor, e.g. 'sharp cheese' or 'loud shirt'. There are also hints that patients with right hemisphere lesions show problems with metaphor. It is possible that their deficits are mainly with spatial metaphors, such as 'He stepped down as director'.[10]

The fact that the angular gyrus is proportionately much larger in hominids than other primates, and its strategic location at the crossroads of areas specialized for processing touch, hearing and vision, leads Ramachandran to believe that it is critical both to conceptual metaphors and to cross-modal abstractions more generally. However, recent research challenges this theory.

Research by Krish Sathian (Emory University) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) suggests that the angular gyrus does not play a role in creating conceptual metaphors. Sathian theorizes that conceptual metaphors activate the texture-selective somatosensory cortex in the parietal operculum.[11]

Brownsett and Wise highlight the role of the left angular gyrus in both speaking and writing.[12]

Arithmetic and spatial cognition

[edit]

Since 1919, brain injuries to the angular gyrus have been known to often cause arithmetic deficits.[13][14] Functional imaging has shown that while other parts of the parietal lobe bilaterally are involved in approximate calculations due to its link with spatiovisual abilities, the left angular gyrus together with left Inferior frontal gyrus are involved in exact calculation due to verbal arithmetic fact retrieval.[15] When activation in the left angular gyrus is greater, a person's arithmetic skills are also more competent.[16]

Attention

[edit]

The right angular gyrus has been associated with spatiovisual attention toward salient features.[17][18] It may allocate attention by employing a bottom-up strategy which draws on the area's ability to attend to retrieved memories.[17] For example, the angular gyrus plays a critical role in distinguishing left from right by integrating the conceptual understanding of the language term "left" or "right" with its location in space.[19] Furthermore, the angular gyrus has been associated with orienting in three dimensional space, not because it interprets space, but because it may control attention shifts in space.[20]

Other functions

[edit]

Default mode network

[edit]

The angular gyrus is part of the default mode network, a network of brain regions activated during multi-modal activities that are independent of external stimuli.[21][22]

Awareness

[edit]

The angular gyrus reacts differently to intended and consequential movement.[23] This suggests that the angular gyrus monitors the self's intended movements and uses the added information to compute differently, as it does for consequential movements. By recording the discrepancy, the angular gyrus maintains an awareness of the self.

Memory retrieval

[edit]

Activation of the angular gyrus shows that not only does it mediate memory retrieval, but it also notes contradictions between what is expected from the retrieval, and what is unusual.[3] The angular gyrus can access both content and episodic memories and is useful in inferring from these the intentions of human characters.[17] Furthermore, the angular gyrus may use a feedback strategy to ascertain whether a retrieval is expected or unusual.

Out-of-body experiences

[edit]

Experiments have demonstrated the ability of stimulation of the right angular gyrus to induce out-of-body experiences.[24] Stimulation of the left angular gyrus in one experiment caused a woman to perceive a shadowy person lurking behind her. The shadowy figure is actually a perceived double of the self.[25] Another such experiment gave the test subject the sensation of being on the ceiling. This is attributed to a discrepancy in the actual position of the body, and the mind's perceived location of the body.

Clinical significance

[edit]

Damage to the angular gyrus manifests as Gerstmann syndrome. Damage may impair one or more of the below functions.

  1. Dysgraphia/agraphia: deficiency in the ability to write[26][27]
  2. Dyscalculia/acalculia: difficulty in learning or comprehending mathematics[26][27]
  3. Finger agnosia: inability to distinguish the fingers on the hand[26][27]
  4. Left-right disorientation[26][27]

Additional images

[edit]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Seghier, Mohamed L. (February 2013). "The Angular Gyrus". The Neuroscientist. 19 (1): 43–61. doi:10.1177/1073858412440596. ISSN 1073-8584. PMC 4107834. PMID 22547530.
  2. ^ John, Hall (2010). Guyton and Hall Textbook of Medical Physiology. Saunders. p. 699. ISBN 978-1416045748.
  3. ^ a b Park, HJ; Kim, JJ; Lee, SK; Seok, JH; Chun, J; Kim, DI; et al. (2008). "Corpus callosal connection mapping using cortical gray matter parcellation and DT-MRI". Human Brain Mapping. 29 (5): 503–16. doi:10.1002/hbm.20314. PMC 6870924. PMID 17133394.
  4. ^ Makris, Nikos; Kennedy, David N.; McInerney, Sean; Sorensen, A. Gregory; Wang, Ruopeng; Verne, S. Caviness Jr; Pandya, Deepak N. (2005). "Segmentation of Subcomponents within the Superior Longitudinal Fascicle in Humans: A Quantitative, In Vivo, DT-MRI Study". Cereb. Cortex. 15 (6): 854–869. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh186. PMID 15590909.
  5. ^ a b Uddin, Lucina Q.; Supekar, Kaustubh; Amin, Hitha; Rykhlevskaia, Elena; Nguyen, Daniel A.; Greicius, Michael D.; Menon, Vinod (2010). "Dissociable Connectivity within Human Angular Gyrus and Intraparietal Sulcus: Evidence from Functional and Structural Connectivity". Cereb. Cortex. 20 (11): 2636–2646. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq011. PMC 2951845. PMID 20154013.
  6. ^ Rushworth, MF; Behrens, TE; Johansen-Berg, H (2006). "Connection patterns distinguish 3 regions of human parietal cortex". Cereb Cortex. 16 (10): 1418–1430. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.504.2825. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj079. PMID 16306320.
  7. ^ Makris, Nikos; Papadimitriou, George M.; Sorg, Scott; Kennedy, David N.; Caviness, Verne S.; Pandya, Deepak N. (2007). "The occipitofrontal fascicle in humans: A quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study". NeuroImage. 37 (4): 1100–1111. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.042. PMC 3769215. PMID 17681797.
  8. ^ Lee, H; Devlin, JT; Shakeshaft, C; Stewart, LH; Brennan, A; Glensman, J; Pitcher, K; Crinion, J; Mechelli, A; Frackowiak, RS; Green, DW; Price, CJ (2007). "Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain" (PDF). J Neurosci. 27 (5): 1184–1189. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4442-06.2007. PMC 6673201. PMID 17267574.
  9. ^ Gai, Dayu. "Angular gyrus | Radiology Reference Article | Radiopaedia.org". Radiopaedia. doi:10.53347/rid-38811. Retrieved 2022-06-15.
  10. ^ Ramachandran, V.S.; Hubbard, E.M (2003). "The Phenomenology of Synaesthesia" (PDF). Journal of Consciousness Studies. 10 (8): 49–57.
  11. ^ Simon, K; Stilla, R; Sathian, K (2011). "Metaphorically feeling:Comprehending textual metaphors actives somatosensory cortex". Brain and Language. 120 (3): 416–421. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.016. PMC 3318916. PMID 22305051.
  12. ^ Brownsett, Sonia L. E.; Wise, Richard J. S. (2009-06-16). "The Contribution of the Parietal Lobes to Speaking and Writing". Cerebral Cortex. 20 (3): 517–523. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp120. ISSN 1460-2199. PMC 2820696. PMID 19531538.
  13. ^ Henschen, SL (1919). "On language, music and calculation mechanisms and their localisation in the cerebrum". Zeitschrift für die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie. 52: 273–298. doi:10.1007/bf02872428. S2CID 123138541.
  14. ^ Gerstmann, J (1940). "Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia and acalculia—Local diagnostic value". Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 44 (2): 398–408. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1940.02280080158009.
  15. ^ Dehaene, S; Spelke, E; Pinel, P; Stanescu, R; Tsivkin, S (1999). "Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence". Science. 284 (5416): 970–4. Bibcode:1999Sci...284..970D. doi:10.1126/science.284.5416.970. PMID 10320379.
  16. ^ Grabner, RH; Ansari, D; Reishofer, G; Stern, E; Ebner, F; Neuper, C (2007). "Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation". NeuroImage. 38 (2): 346–56. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.041. PMID 17851092. S2CID 18960661.
  17. ^ a b c Seghier, M. L. (2012). "The angular gyrus: multiple function ad multiple subdivisions". Neuroscientist. 19 (1): 43–61. doi:10.1177/1073858412440596. PMC 4107834. PMID 22547530.
  18. ^ Arsalidou, M; Taylor, MJ (2011). "Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations". NeuroImage. 54 (3): 2382–93. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009. PMID 20946958. S2CID 140205603.
  19. ^ Hirnstein, M; Bayer, U; Ellison, A; Hausmann, M (2011). "TMS over the left angular gyrus impairs the ability to discriminate left from right". Neuropsychologia. 49 (1): 29–33. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.028. PMID 21035475. S2CID 8460451.
  20. ^ Chen, Q; Weidner, R; Vossel, S; Weiss, PH; Fink, GR (Sep 2012). "Neural mechanisms of attentional reorienting in three-dimensional space". J Neurosci. 32 (39): 13352–62. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1772-12.2012. PMC 6621370. PMID 23015426.
  21. ^ Murphy, Charlotte; Jefferies, Elizabeth; Rueschemeyer, Shirley-Ann; Sormaz, Mladen; Wang, Hao-ting; Margulies, Daniel S.; Smallwood, Jonathan (2018-05-01). "Distant from input: Evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceptually-guided cognition". NeuroImage. 171: 393–401. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.017. ISSN 1053-8119. PMC 5883322. PMID 29339310.
  22. ^ Vatansever, D.; Manktelow, A. E.; Sahakian, B. J.; Menon, D. K.; Stamatakis, E. A. (January 2017). "Angular default mode network connectivity across working memory load". Human Brain Mapping. 38 (1): 41–52. doi:10.1002/hbm.23341. ISSN 1097-0193. PMC 6866899. PMID 27489137.
  23. ^ Farrer C, Frey SH, Van Horn JD, Tunik E, Turk D, Inati S, Grafton ST. The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations. Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive.
  24. ^ Out-of-Body Experience? Your Brain Is to Blame - New York Times
  25. ^ Arzy, S.; Seeck, M.; Ortigue, S.; Spinelli, L.; Blanke, O. (2006). "Induction of an illusory shadow person: Stimulation of a site on the brain's left hemisphere prompts the creepy feeling that somebody is close by". Nature. 443 (21): 287. doi:10.1038/443287a. PMID 16988702. S2CID 4338465.
  26. ^ a b c d Vallar G (July 2007). "Spatial neglect, Balint-Homes' and Gerstmann's syndrome, and other spatial disorders". CNS Spectr. 12 (7): 527–36. doi:10.1017/S1092852900021271. PMID 17603404. S2CID 45201083.
  27. ^ a b c d Carota A, Di Pietro M, Ptak R, Poglia D, Schnider A (2004). "Defective spatial imagery with pure Gerstmann's syndrome". Eur. Neurol. 52 (1): 1–6. doi:10.1159/000079251. PMID 15218337. S2CID 22897847.
[edit]