Biological tests of necessity and sufficiency
The topic of this article may not meet Wikipedia's general notability guideline. (December 2021) |
Biological tests of necessity and sufficiency refer to experimental methods and techniques that seek to test or provide evidence for specific kinds of causal relationships in biological systems. A necessary cause is one without which it would be impossible for an effect to occur, while a sufficient cause is one whose presence guarantees the occurrence of an effect. These concepts are largely based on but distinct from ideas of necessity and sufficiency in logic.
Tests of necessity, among which are methods of lesioning or gene knockout, and tests of sufficiency, among which are methods of isolation or discrete stimulation of factors, have become important in current-day experimental designs, and application of these tests have led to a number of notable discoveries and findings in the biological sciences.
Definitions[edit]
In biological research, experiments or tests are often used to study predicted causal relationships between two phenomena.[1] These causal relationships may be described in terms of the logical concepts of necessity and sufficiency.
Consider the statement that a phenomenon x causes a phenomenon y. X would be a necessary cause of y when the occurrence of y implies that x needed to have occurred. However, only the occurrence of the necessary condition x may not always result in y also occurring.[2] In other words, when some factor is necessary to cause an effect, it is impossible to have the effect without the cause.[3] X would instead be a sufficient cause of y when the occurrence of x implies that y must then occur.[2] in other words, when some factor is sufficient to cause an effect, the presence of the cause guarantees the occurrence of the effect.[3] However, a different cause z may also cause y, meaning that y may occur without x occurring.[2][3] For a concrete example, consider the conditional statement "if an object is a square, then it has four sides". It is a necessary condition that an object has four sides if it is true that it is a square; conversely, the object being a square is a sufficient condition for it to be true that an object has four sides.[4]
Four distinct combinations of necessity and sufficiency are possible for a relationship of two conditions. A first condition may be:
- necessary but not sufficient
- sufficient but not necessary
- both necessary and sufficient
- neither necessary nor sufficient
for a second condition to be true.[4]
Laypersons or novice scientists may prefer to think of these concepts of necessity and sufficiency in certain ways. In a study of undergraduate psychology students, more students viewed necessity in negative terms of "if no x, then no y" rather than the logically equivalent "if y, then x" than they did for sufficiency; most of them understood sufficiency as "if x, then y" rather than the logically equivalent "if no y, then no x". This result also suggested the students were more likely to prefer thinking about causes in the forward (x to y) direction rather than the backward (y to x) direction.[5]
Misunderstandings may occur regarding the kinds of evidence that can support these causal relationships; a study applying the causal concepts of necessity and sufficiency found that college students in a biochemistry class were likely to erroneously misinterpret correlations as signifying sufficiency to cause an effect.[6] The study on undergraduate psychology students also indicated a potential discrepancy in understanding sufficiency, where it was found that some students understood a cause as being sufficient for an effect even when the effect did not always follow the cause;[5] while in the reverse direction, an effect may have alternative causes, but a cause can only be sufficient if it always results in the effect occurring.
Tests of necessity[edit]
Necessity can only be proven by demonstrating that a system does not work when the piece in question is not active, meaning that it is required for functioning. Biological research uses a variety of techniques to inhibit a structure or genetic player, such as lesions and knockout/deletion. All of these techniques fall into the category of loss of function.[citation needed]
Lesioning[edit]
Lesioning is a test of necessity involves physically damaging a structure so that it loses its function. If lesioning a structure causes a change in the system, then that structure is necessary.[7] Lesioning can look very different across scientific disciplines.
In psychology, information about necessity may be gleaned by observing changes in behavior when a brain region has been destroyed, either by accident or illness in a human or purposefully in a lab animal.[8] Other disciplines may target specific cell types and tag them for degradation. The GAL4/UAS System "reports" a subset of genes of interest although it has no independent effect on tissue.[9] When combined with an apoptotic gene, such as reaper (rpr), all cells that express the genes of interest along with GAL4/UAS, will initiate cell death, creating a legion specific to a cell type.[10]
A classic example of lesioning in chronobiology is the exploration of circadian navigation in monarch butterflies. In a landmark experiment, Dr. Christine Merlin and Dr. Steven Reppert removed (lesioned) the antennae of monarch butterflies and tested their ability to navigate.[11] They found that the Monarchs could not navigate without their antennae.[11] The investigators’ use of lesioning allowed them to come to the conclusion that Monarch's antennae are necessary for navigation because their absence prevented this system from functioning.[11]
Knockout[edit]
A knockout refers to the inactivation of a gene. This can be done several ways, such as altering the DNA sequence so that it no longer performs its function, altering the promoter region so that it is not transcribed, or deleting the gene entirely.[12] Aside from helping scientists understand the function of gene, observing behavior or some other variable before and after deletion can create an argument for its necessity if a behavior or system ceases to function.
Allada et al. performed a knockout of the drosophila gene CLOCK, a gene earlier identified as a circadian gene via forward genetics. In this experiment, they altered the Clock gene to stop it from performing its normal function.[13] Clock knockout flies did not exhibit a twenty-four hour rhythm as was recorded for unchanged flies.[13] This led the researchers to conclude that Clock is necessary for the twenty four hour rhythm of flies.[13]
Tests of sufficiency[edit]
Sufficiency can be proven by demonstrating that a system can operate when the element in question is active without the influence of other elements. However, this does not indicate that the element in question is required for functioning. The element allows the system to function, but its presence is not required in order for the system to function.
Tests of sufficiency in biology are used to determine if the presence of an element permits the biological phenomenon to occur. In other words, if sufficient conditions are met, the targeted event is able to take place. However, this does not mean that the absence of a sufficient biological element inhibits the biological event from occurring.
Tests of sufficiency include a gain of function by discrete stimulation and isolation of a biological element to observe a change in the targeted event. These types of methods are imperative to several subfields of biology. Particularly, tests of sufficiency are common in the field of chronobiology.
Discrete stimulation[edit]
When performing discrete stimulation, researchers aim to provide stimuli solely to the biological element of interest. Returning to Dr. Reppert and Dr. Merlin's work with monarch butterflies, when inquiring about the role of antennas in the monarch butterfly's sun compass orientation, the researchers only provided light to the antennas of the butterfly.[11] They found that stimulating the antennas alone was enough for the butterflies to entrain comparably to non-stimulated butterflies who were entrained in the same condition. As a result, the flight behavior of the stimulated group was similar to butterflies who were traditionally entrained. As a result, there was evidence that the antennas are sufficient for sun compass orientation.
Discrete stimulation has also been used to investigate the location and quantity of the circadian pacemakers in plants, namely Arabidopsis and the tobacco plant.[14] When investigating the coupling of the circadian systems in the intact plants, Thain et al. placed foil covering over one cotyledon and entrained the other uncovered cotyledon to a light-dark cycle of 12 hours light and 12 hours dark (LD 12:12), 6 hours ahead of the covered cotyledon. Then, the researchers covered the opposite cotyledon and entrained the newly exposed cotyledon to a 12:12 light-dark cycle delayed by 6 hours, creating a 12-hour difference between the cotyledons. Using transgenic bioluminescence, Thain et al. discovered that the two leaves were out of phase with each other, displaying peaks in luminescence at opposite times. This provided evidence for the notion that each cotyledon is sufficient for maintaining an independent circadian rhythm and has its own autonomous circadian oscillator.[14]
Isolation[edit]
The method of isolation to determine sufficiency involves a single biological element undergoing experimentation, being secluded from interactions with other relevant biological elements. This method includes the removal of the element in question and placing it into secluded testing conditions.
Reppert and Merlin used the method of isolation in their research in when examining the influence of circadian clock outputs of the antennas on the sun-compass orientation of monarch butterflies in 2009. The researchers separated the antennas from the butterfly and placed them in separate conditions. Then, the researchers proceeded patch clamp onto a cell in the antenna and puff odorant onto the isolated cell to determine if there is rhythmicity in its firing rate.[11] They concluded that it does show rhythmicity in its response to the odorant, with its firing rate being high at night and low in the day.[11] The isolation of the antenna proves its sufficiency because it is the only relevant biological element sustaining the rhythm in the system.
Additionally, they utilizes this approach again in their research in 2012 examining the influence of the temporal relationship between antennas of the monarch butterfly on its sun-compass orientation. The researchers analyzed the importance of the presence of both antennas for sun-compass orientation. They tested this by removing a single antenna from the butterfly and assessing its flight behavior. As a result, they found that one antenna was sufficient for correct sun compass orientation, regardless of its laterality.[15]
The method of isolation is not limited to isolating single cells or organs for experimentation. It can be used to analyze proteins and their role in an organism as well. Namely, Nakajima et al. utilizes this in 2005 while working with Kai proteins such as KaiA, KaiB, KaiC, which regulate circadian gene expression in cyanobacteria. These proteins are major components of the cyanobacteria circadian clock, altering their conformation and phosphorylation state throughout the day.[16] Questioning the transcription-translation feedback loop as a means of explaining the cyanobacterial circadian clock, Nakajima et al. isolated the KaiABC proteins by lysing S. elongatus and placing them in test tubes with ATP. Consequently, in vitro phosphorylation of KaiC oscillated with a period of approximately 24 hours at slightly smaller amplitude than the in vivo oscillation. This demonstrated that the KaiABC proteins are sufficient for maintaining circadian rhythm when provided with ATP. It also presented KaiABC as the circadian post-translation oscillator that regulates the clock in S. elongatus, in tandem with transcription-translation feedback loop (TTFL) model as mechanism for the organism's clock.[17]
Critiques[edit]
The use of the concepts of necessity and sufficiency in experimental design and research interpretations has been criticized on the grounds that the way they are used may not always be consistent with the meanings of these concepts in formal logic. In particular, Yoshihara and Yoshihara have criticized the usage of the phrase "necessary and sufficient" to describe biological factors in research. They argue that researchers who use that phrase to describe the importance of one factor for another run the risk of miscommunicating their findings to those who interpret the phrase in a logical sense, but at the same time, citing the example of how command neurons are formally defined, that applying the idea of "necessary and sufficient" to biological factors in a strictly logical sense may be overly restrictive and result in some findings being inappropriately dismissed. As an alternative description, the authors suggest using the phrase "indispensable and inducing".[18][19]
References[edit]
- ^ Chiang IA, Jhangiani RS, Price PC (2015). "Experiment Basics". Research Methods in Psychology - 2nd Canadian Edition. Victoria, B.C.: BCcampus. ISBN 978-1-77420-011-7.
- ^ Jump up to: a b c "Causal Reasoning". Retrieved 2021-04-22.
- ^ Jump up to: a b c "[M06] Necessity and sufficiency". philosophy.hku.hk. Retrieved 2021-05-06.
- ^ Jump up to: a b Swartz N (1997). "The Concepts of Necessary Conditions and Sufficient Conditions". Archived from the original on 2010-12-21. Retrieved 2021-04-22.
- ^ Jump up to: a b Verschueren N, Schroyens W, Schaeken W, d'Ydewalle G (2004-09-01). "The interpretation of the concepts 'necessity' and 'sufficiency' in forward unicausal relations". Current Psychology Letters. Behaviour, Brain & Cognition (14, Vol. 3, 2004). doi:10.4000/cpl.433. ISSN 1379-6100.
- ^ Coleman AB, Lam DP, Soowal LN (2015). "Correlation, necessity, and sufficiency: Common errors in the scientific reasoning of undergraduate students for interpreting experiments". Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Education. 43 (5): 305–15. doi:10.1002/bmb.20879. PMID 26146931. S2CID 205519318.
- ^ Vaidya AR, Pujara MS, Petrides M, Murray EA, Fellows LK (August 2019). "Lesion Studies in Contemporary Neuroscience". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 23 (8): 653–671. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.009. PMC 6712987. PMID 31279672.
- ^ Lavond DG, Steinmetz JE (2003). "Lesion Techniques for Behavioral Experiments". In Lavond DG, Steinmetz JE (eds.). Handbook of Classical Conditioning. Boston, MA: Springer US. pp. 249–276. doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-0263-0_8. ISBN 978-1-4615-0263-0.
- ^ Wyart C, Del Bene F, Warp E, Scott EK, Trauner D, Baier H, Isacoff EY (September 2009). "Optogenetic dissection of a behavioural module in the vertebrate spinal cord". Nature. 461 (7262): 407–10. Bibcode:2009Natur.461..407W. doi:10.1038/nature08323. PMC 2770190. PMID 19759620.
- ^ White K, Tahaoglu E, Steller H (February 1996). "Cell killing by the Drosophila gene reaper". Science. 271 (5250): 805–7. Bibcode:1996Sci...271..805W. doi:10.1126/science.271.5250.805. PMID 8628996. S2CID 20522069.
- ^ Jump up to: a b c d e f Merlin C, Gegear RJ, Reppert SM (September 2009). "Antennal circadian clocks coordinate sun compass orientation in migratory monarch butterflies". Science. 325 (5948): 1700–4. Bibcode:2009Sci...325.1700M. doi:10.1126/science.1176221. PMC 2754321. PMID 19779201.
- ^ "Gene Knockout: Steps, Methods and Applications". Genetic Education. 2019-12-02. Retrieved 2021-04-21.
- ^ Jump up to: a b c Allada R, White NE, So WV, Hall JC, Rosbash M (May 1998). "A mutant Drosophila homolog of mammalian Clock disrupts circadian rhythms and transcription of period and timeless". Cell. 93 (5): 791–804. doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81440-3. PMID 9630223. S2CID 1779880.
- ^ Jump up to: a b Thain SC, Hall A, Millar AJ (August 2000). "Functional independence of circadian clocks that regulate plant gene expression". Current Biology. 10 (16): 951–6. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00630-8. PMID 10985381. S2CID 14438917.
- ^ Guerra PA, Merlin C, Gegear RJ, Reppert SM (July 2012). "Discordant timing between antennae disrupts sun compass orientation in migratory monarch butterflies". Nature Communications. 3: 958. Bibcode:2012NatCo...3..958G. doi:10.1038/ncomms1965. PMC 3962218. PMID 22805565.
- ^ Cohen SE, Golden SS (December 2015). "Circadian Rhythms in Cyanobacteria". Microbiology and Molecular Biology Reviews. 79 (4): 373–85. doi:10.1128/MMBR.00036-15. PMC 4557074. PMID 26335718.
- ^ Nakajima M, Imai K, Ito H, Nishiwaki T, Murayama Y, Iwasaki H, et al. (April 2005). "Reconstitution of circadian oscillation of cyanobacterial KaiC phosphorylation in vitro". Science. 308 (5720): 414–5. Bibcode:2005Sci...308..414N. doi:10.1126/science.1108451. PMID 15831759. S2CID 24833877.
- ^ Yoshihara M, Yoshihara M (2018-04-03). "'Necessary and sufficient' in biology is not necessarily necessary - confusions and erroneous conclusions resulting from misapplied logic in the field of biology, especially neuroscience". Journal of Neurogenetics. 32 (2): 53–64. doi:10.1080/01677063.2018.1468443. PMC 6510664. PMID 29757057.
- ^ "The phrase 'necessary and sufficient' blamed for flawed neuroscience". Nature. 558 (7709): 162. June 2018. Bibcode:2018Natur.558R.162.. doi:10.1038/d41586-018-05418-0. PMID 29895927. S2CID 48354584.