Jump to content

Schaly Brrasted Munia

Это хорошая статья. Нажмите здесь для получения дополнительной информации.
(Перенаправлено из Lonchura Punctulata )

Schaly Brrasted Munia
Л. П. Punctulata ( Индия )
Научная классификация Измените эту классификацию
Домен: Эукариота
Королевство: Животное
Филум: Chordata
Сорт: Птица
Заказ: Прохожий
Семья: Estrildidae
Род: Lonchura
Разновидность:
L. Point
Биномиальное название
Пунктурный Lonchura
Местный диапазон
Местный диапазон
Синонимы
  • Loxia punctulata 1758

Шешенистая муния или пятнистая муния ( Lonchura Punctulata ), известная в торговле домашних животных как мускатный орех Mannikin или Spice Finch , является воробьем , родом из эстральдидного зяблика в тропической Азии. Вид рода Lonchura , он был официально описан и назван Карлом Линнеусом в 1758 году. Его название основано на различных масштабных маркировках перьев на груди и животе. Взрослый коричневый выше и имеет темный конический счет. Вид имеет 11 подвидов по всему диапазону, которые немного различаются по размеру и цвету.

Эта муния ест в основном семена травы, кроме ягод и небольших насекомых. Они добывают в стаях и общаются с мягкими звонками и свистками. Вид очень социальный и может иногда укорениться с другими видами мунией. Этот вид встречается на тропических равнинах и лугах . Перецены пары конструируют купольные гнезда, используя траву или бамбуковые листья.

Этот вид эндемичен по отношению к Азии и происходит от Индии и Шри -Ланки на восток до Индонезии и Филиппин (где он называется Mayang Pakíng ). Он был введен во многие другие части мира, и дикие группы населения зарекомендовали себя в Пуэрто -Рико и Хиспаньоле , а также в частях Австралии и Соединенных Штатов Америки с наблюдениями в Калифорнии. Птица перечисляется как наименьшая забота Международным союзом сохранения природы (МСОП).

Таксономия

[ редактировать ]

В 1743 году английский натуралист Джордж Эдвардс включил иллюстрацию и описание чешуйчатой ​​мунии в первом томе своей естественной истории необычных птиц . Он использовал английское имя «Говри птица». Эдвардс основал свое офорта в ручном цвете на образце в лондонском доме Чарльза дю Буа , казначея в Ост-Индской компании . [ 2 ] Когда в 1758 году шведский натуралист Карл Линнеус обновил свою систему Naturae для десятого издания , он поместил заглашенную мунию с перекрестными путями в роду Loxia . Linnaeus включил краткое описание, придумал биномиальное название Loxia punctulata и процитировала работу Эдвардса. [ 3 ] Линнеус указал место как «Азию», но это было ограничено Калькуттой (Калькутта) Е.С. Стюартом Бейкером в 1926 году. [ 4 ] [ 5 ] В настоящее время этот вид помещается в род Lonchura , который был представлен английским натуралистом Уильямом Генри Сайксом в 1832 году. [ 6 ] [ 7 ] Название рода Lonchura сочетает в себе древнегреческий лонххи, означающий «копья-головы» или «копья» с Oura, означающим «хвост». Конкретный эпитет из современного латинского пунктулатуса, означающего «пятнистый» или «пунктирный». [ 8 ]

В большом диапазоне существует 11 признанных подвидов . К ним относятся форма назначения , обнаруженную на равнинах индийского субконтинента , включая Пакистан, Индию, Иран, Непал, Бангладеш и Шри -Ланка. Название Lineoventer раньше использовалось для индийского населения. Другие популяции включают в себя субиндураты из восточных Гималаев, Юннаненсис из южного Китая, Тпель -Таиланд, Кабаниси Филиппин и фрутис Сингапура и Суматра. Популяции островов включают Низорию (Java, Bali, Lombok, Sumbawa), Particeps (Sulawesi), Baweana (остров Бавин), Sumbae (Sumba) и Blasii (Flores, Timor и Tanimbar). [ 7 ]

The subspecies holmesi (southeast Borneo) is sometimes recognised.[9]

Description

[edit]
juvenile L. p. punctulata, Sri Lanka

The scaly-breasted munia is about 11–12 centimetres (4.3–4.7 in) long and weighs 12–16 grams (0.026–0.035 lb). The adult has a stubby dark bill typical of grain eating birds, brown upperparts and a dark brown head. The underparts are white with dark scale markings. The sexes are similar, although males have darker markings on the underside and a darker throat than females.[10]

Immature birds have pale brown upperparts, lack the dark head found in adults, and have uniform buff underparts that can be confused with juveniles of other munia species such as the tricolored munia (Lonchura malacca) across the Asian and island populations and the black-throated munia (Lonchura kelaarti) in parts of India or Sri Lanka.[10][11]

Distribution and habitat

[edit]
The scaly-breasted munia (subspecies topela[12]) has established in parts of eastern Australia such as Queensland

Scaly-breasted munias are found in a range of habitats but are usually close to water and grassland. In India, they are especially common in paddy fields where they are considered a minor pest on account of their feeding on grain. They are found mainly on the plains, but can be observed in the foothills of the Himalayas, in which they may be present at altitudes near 2,500 m (1.6 mi), and in the Nilgiris, where they are found at altitudes up to 2,100 m (6,900 ft) during the summer. In Pakistan, they are restricted to a narrow region from Swat in the west to Lahore, avoiding the desert zone, and then occurring again in India east of an area between Ludhiana and Mount Abu.[13] The species has also been observed in Kashmir, though this is rare.[14][15]

Outside their native range, escaped birds frequently establish themselves in areas with a suitable climate and can then colonize new areas nearby. Escaped cage-birds established in the wild and such populations have been recorded in the West Indies (Puerto Rico since 1971),[16] Hawaii (since 1883[17]),[18] Japan[19] and southern United States, mainly in Florida and California.[20][21] In Oahu, Hawaii, they compete for habitats with the tricolored munia and tend to be rare where this competitor is present.[18] The species has been introduced to other parts of the world due to its popularity as a cage bird and populations have established in the wild.[22][23]

Behaviour and ecology

[edit]

Sociality

[edit]

Scaly-breasted munias form flocks of as many as 100 birds. Individuals communicate with calls that include a short whistle, variations of kitty-kitty-kitty, and a sharp chipping alarm note.[11][23] They sometimes flick their tails and wings vertically or horizontally while hopping about. The tail flicking motion may have evolved from a locomotory intention movement. The exaggerated version of the tail flicking movement may have undergone ritualization. As a social signal, tail flicking in several other species acts as a signal indicating the intent to fly and helps keep flocks together.[23][24]

When roosting communally, scaly-breasted munia sit side by side in close contact with each other. The outermost bird often jostles towards the center. Birds in a flock sometimes preen each other, with the soliciting bird usually showing its chin. Allopreening is usually limited to the face and neck.[23] The scaly-breasted munia is rarely hostile but birds will sometimes quarrel without any ritualized posturing.[23]

Breeding

[edit]
Egg of scaly-breasted munia

The breeding season is during the summer rainy season (mainly June to August and also in October season in India) but can vary. Laboratory studies have found that long day illumination and high humidity trigger gonadal growth.[25] The song of the male is very soft but complex and variable, audible only at close range. This song described as a jingle consists of a series of high notes followed by a croaky rattle and ending in a slurred whistle. When singing the male sits in what is called the slope posture—erect with the head feathers raised.[23]

There are two types of slope posture, a pre-copulatory one and an ordinary one. The pre-copulatory behavior of scaly-breasted munia includes a sequence of actions. The first involves either the male or female playing with nest-material. As soon as a bird has arranged the nest material in its bill, it begins to fly around in a zigzag path. Once a bird lands close to its partner, the male bends towards the female and wipes its bill. The male then sings with movements of the body. The female invites mounting with tail quivering.[11][23] The nest is a large domed structure loosely woven from blades of grass, bamboo or other leaves with a side entrance and is placed in a tree or under the eaves of a house. A study in southern India found the preferred nesting trees to be Toddalia asiatica, Gymnosporia montana and Acacia chundra, especially short and bushy ones in areas with low canopy cover. The nest opening is located to face downwind of the most frequent wind direction.[26] In northern India, they preferred isolated Acacia nilotica in non-urban areas but used Thuja orientalis and Polyalthia longifolia in urban gardens.[27]

Scaly-breasted munia clutches usually contain 4 to 6 eggs, but can contain up to 10. Both sexes build the nest and incubate the eggs, which hatch in 10 to 16 days.[14][28]

The species is extensively used as a brood host by the parasitic pin-tailed whydah in Southern California — where both species are feral — with the munia raising the whydah's chicks as its own.[29] This relationship is novel, as the two species do not naturally co-occur in their native ranges, and had no established evolutionary relationship as parasite and host.[29]

Food and foraging

[edit]
Adult feeding young

The scaly-breasted munia feeds mainly on grass seeds, small berries such as those of Lantana and insects.[30] Although the bill is suited for crushing small grains, they do not show lateral movements of the lower mandible which help European greenfinches in dehusking seeds.[31] Like some other munias, they may also feed on algae, a rich protein source, prior to the breeding season.[32]

The ease of maintaining these birds in captivity has made them popular for studying behavior and physiology. Feeding behavior can be predicted by the optimal foraging theory, where animals minimize time and energy spent to maximize food intake. This theory has been tested by studying the strategies used by scaly-breasted munias to increase their feeding efficacy.[33]

Flock size tradeoffs

[edit]

Studies on foraging have examined the effect of group size in reducing time spent on predator vigilance, thereby increasing feeding efficiency. According to the "many-eyes" hypothesis,[34] a reduction in the individual time spent on vigilance against threats in larger groups allows for more time to be spent on searching for food and feeding. Vigilance is greatest among solitary individuals and reduces as the group size increases to about four. The birds collect seeds more quickly in larger groups, reflecting a decrease in individual vigilance, a decrease in handling time, and an increase in both search speed and focus when foraging.[35]

A foraging group

Individuals may also take advantage of group foraging by "joining" members that have found food. The options to seek food or to join others that have discovered food involves information sharing and has been studied through what are termed "producer-scrounger models".[36] A cost associated with group foraging is increased resource competition, which in turn may reduce anti-predatory vigilance due to the intensity of foraging.[37] Some studies show that increased competition results in a decreased feeding rate.[38]

Foraging models

[edit]

When foraging, scaly-breasted munia can search as individuals or search for others that have found food and join them. The economic consequences of the decision to join others has been modeled in two ways: the producer-scrounger model and the information sharing model. These models are based on hypotheses that differ in the degree of compatibility that is assumed between the two food and joining opportunity search modes.[39]

The information sharing model assumes that individuals search concurrently for finding and joining opportunities while the producer-scrounger model assumes that the search modes are mutually exclusive.[39] Hopping with the head facing up and downward are observed to be statistically associated with the frequencies of a bird's joining and finding, respectively. When the expected stable frequency of the scrounger tactic was altered by changing the availability of seeds, the relative frequency of hopping with the head up changed accordingly. When the seed distribution made the scrounger tactic unprofitable, the frequency of hopping with the head up diminished and appears to support the predictions of the producer-scrounger model.[40]

Studies show that scaly-breasted munias tend to adopt the scrounger tactic when food is more clumped and when the group size increases. When most foragers adopt scrounging, the time taken to discover new food patches is greater.[41]

Vigilance

[edit]

Most social foragers must search for food while also avoiding predators. It has been suggested that individuals that play scrounger could also, by virtue of their head position, be alert for predators and hence contribute to antipredatory vigilance. If the scrounger tactic is compatible with antipredatory vigilance, then an increase in antipredatory vigilance should lead to the detection of more joining opportunities, and hence more joining. When stationary, the head-up tactic has been shown to be associated with antipredatory vigilance. However scanning while hopping does not aid in vigilance and it is thought that the scrounger tactic is incompatible with antipredatory vigilance in the scaly-breasted munia.[42]

Specialized foraging

[edit]

Scaly-breasted munias have variable competitive behaviors that allow them to exploit scarce resources. There are two foraging alternatives: producers that make the food available and scroungers that steal food found by the producers. Studies show that these choices lead to a stable equilibrium within a group. When individuals are free to choose between producer and scrounger, frequency dependent selection results in a stable mixture of both behaviors where each receives similar payoff. Studies indicate that if most of the population consists of producers, then scrounging behavior is favored by natural selection because there is plenty of food to steal. On the other hand, if most birds exhibit scrounging then the competition for stealing is so great that producing is favored.[43][44]

A pair feeding on grains

Three hypotheses might account for consistent foraging specializations across individuals: food source variation, phenotypic differences, and frequency dependent-choice. The food source variation hypothesis predicts that individuals will specialize when the use of two skills is more costly than specialist foraging. The phenotypic differences hypothesis proposes that individuals differ in their ability to use each foraging skill and stably specialize on the most profitable one. The pattern of specialization is expected to be stable although the number of individuals that use a given skill depends on the phenotypic composition of the flock. The frequency dependent choice hypothesis also proposes that individuals specialize on the most profitable skill, but the profitability of each alternative decreases as the number of phenotypically identical foragers gradually specialize on each skill when initially given two equally profitable alternatives. At equilibrium, individual payoffs should be independent of the pattern of specialization. Individuals in flocks adjusted their use of the two skills and two birds in each flock specialized on a different skill resulting in a variant of both the food source variation hypothesis and frequency dependent choice hypothesis.[45]

Aviary experiments conducted with captive flocks of scaly-breasted munia have tested whether producers and scroungers reach the predicted stable equilibrium frequency (see Evolutionarily stable strategy) when individuals are free to choose either behavior. The numbers choosing either producers and scrounger strategies have been shown to converge on stable frequencies while demonstrating that variation in tactics arise through frequency dependent pay-offs from the choice of different feeding strategies.[46]

Furthermore, foraging birds may feed actively on the substrate or pick grains dropped on the ground and these strategies may be chosen according to the situation. Early departures occur more often when expected searching time decreases and when competition intensity increases. Competition intensity is expected to increase when more scroungers are present or when patches are smaller.[47]

Prey crypsis

[edit]

Since producers search for food and scroungers wait for opportunities to join, prey crypsis imposes a producer specific cost that shifts the producer scrounger equilibria towards more scrounging. Prey crypsis resulted in increased latency to eat the seed and increased number of detection errors.[48] Moreover, the presence of a competitor negatively affected foraging efficiency under cryptic backgrounds. The foraging efficiency of individuals that had previously foraged with a competitor on cryptic seeds remained low even after the competitor had been removed. Thus, the costs of foraging on cryptic prey may be greater for social foragers than for solitary foragers.[49]

Resource defence

[edit]

Recent models of economic defence in a group-foraging context predict that the frequency of aggressive interactions should decline as resource density increases.[50][51][52] Studies with scaly-breasted munia show that the intensity of aggressive encounters was highest when patch location was signaled, and the effect of changing resource density depended on whether patch location was signaled or not. Signaling patch location was equivalent to making the resources more spatially predictable. Changing patch density had no effect on the number of aggressive encounters when the location of food was not signaled. When food location was signaled, increasing patch density resulted in the predicted decrease in the number of aggressive encounters.[53]

Conservation

[edit]

The scaly-breasted munia is an abundant species and classified as least concern on the IUCN Red List.[1] The species occupies an extremely large range, and its population, while still unquantified, is large and stable. The scaly-breasted munia is not globally threatened and is common to very common throughout most of its range. However, some populations are dwindled due to the increase of bird cagings.[54]

In many areas it is regarded as an agricultural pest, feeding in large flocks on cultivated cereals such as rice.[55] In Southeast Asia, the scaly-breasted munia is trapped in large numbers for Buddhist ceremonies, but most birds are later released.[56]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Jump up to: a b BirdLife International (2016). "Lonchura punctulata". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2016: e.T22719821A94646304. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-3.RLTS.T22719821A94646304.en. Retrieved 19 November 2021.
  2. ^ Edwards, George (1743). A Natural History of Uncommon Birds. Vol. Part 1. London: Printed for the author at the College of Physicians. p. 40, Plate 40.
  3. ^ Linnaeus, Carl (1758). Systema Naturae per regna tria naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus, differentiis, synonymis, locis (in Latin). Vol. 1 (10th ed.). Holmiae (Stockholm): Laurentii Salvii. p. 173.
  4. ^ Baker, E.C. Stuart (1926). The Fauna of British India Birds including Ceylon and Burma. Birds. Vol. 3 (2nd ed.). London: Taylor and Francis. p. 91.
  5. ^ Paynter, Raymond A. Jr, ed. (1968). Check-List of Birds of the World. Vol. 14. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Museum of Comparative Zoology.
  6. ^ Sykes, William Henry (1832). "Catalogue of birds of the raptorial and insessorial orders (systematically arranged,) observed in the Dukhun". Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London. 2 (18): 77–99 [94].
  7. ^ Jump up to: a b Gill, Frank; Donsker, David; Rasmussen, Pamela, eds. (July 2021). "Waxbills, parrotfinches, munias, whydahs, Olive Warbler, accentors, pipits". IOC World Bird List Version 11.2. International Ornithologists' Union. Retrieved 13 July 2021.
  8. ^ Jobling, J. A. (2010). The Helm Dictionary of Scientific Bird Names. London: Christopher Helm. pp. 229, 324. ISBN 978-1-4081-2501-4.
  9. ^ Clements, J. F.; T. S. Schulenberg; M. J. Iliff; B.L. Sullivan; C. L. Wood & D. Roberson (2013). The eBird/Clements checklist of birds of the world: Version 6.8. The Cornell Lab of Ornithology.
  10. ^ Jump up to: a b Rasmussen, P.C. & Anderton, J.C. (2005). Birds of South Asia. The Ripley Guide. Vol. Volume 2. Smithsonian Institution and Lynx Edicions. p. 673. ISBN 978-84-87334-66-5.
  11. ^ Jump up to: a b c Restall, R. (1997). Munias and Mannikins. Yale University Press. pp. 97–105. ISBN 978-0-300-07109-2.
  12. ^ Forshaw J; Mark Shephard; Anthony Pridham. Grassfinches in Australia. Csiro Publishing. pp. 267–268.
  13. ^ Abbass, D.; Rais, M.; Ghalib, S.A. & Khan, M.Z. (2010). "First Record of Spotted Munia (Lonchura punctulata) from Karachi". Pakistan Journal of Zoology. 42 (4): 503–505.
  14. ^ Jump up to: a b Ali, S. & Ripley, S.D. (1999). Handbook of the Birds of India and Pakistan. Vol. Volume 10 (Second ed.). New Delhi: Oxford University Press. pp. 119–121. ISBN 978-0-19-563708-3.
  15. ^ Akhtar, S.A.; Rao, P.; Tiwari, J.K.; Javed, S. (1992). "Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata (Linn.) from Dachigam National Park, Jammu and Kashmir". Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society. 89 (1): 129.
  16. ^ Moreno, J.A. (1997). "Review of the Subspecific Status and Origin of Introduced Finches in Puerto Rico". Caribbean Journal of Science. 33 (3–4): 233–238.
  17. ^ Moulton, M.P. (1993). "The All-or-None Pattern in Introduced Hawaiian Passeriforms: The role of competition sustained". The American Naturalist. 141 (1): 105–119. doi:10.1086/285463. JSTOR 2462765. S2CID 84341527.
  18. ^ Jump up to: a b Moulton, M. P.; Allen, L. J. S. & Ferris, D. K. (1992). "Competition, resource use and habitat selection in two introduced Hawaiian Mannikins". Biotropica. 24 (1): 77–85. Bibcode:1992Biotr..24...77M. doi:10.2307/2388475. JSTOR 2388475.
  19. ^ Eguchi, K. & Amano, H.E. (2004). "Invasive Birds in Japan" (PDF). Global Environmental Research. 8 (1): 29–39.
  20. ^ Duncan, R.A. (2009). "The status of the nutmeg mannikin (Lonchura punctulata) in the extreme western panhandle of Florida" (PDF). Florida Field Naturalist. 37 (3): 96–97.
  21. ^ Garrett, K.L. (2000). "The juvenile nutmeg mannikin: identification of a little brown bird" (PDF). Western Birds. 31 (2): 130–131.
  22. ^ Burton, M.; Burton, R. (2002). International Wildlife Encyclopedia. New York, NY: Marshal Cavendish. ISBN 9780761472865.
  23. ^ Jump up to: a b c d e f g Moynihan, M. & Hall, M.F. (1954). "Hostile, Sexual, and Other Social Behaviour Patterns of the Spice Finch (Lonchura punctulata) in captivity". Behaviour. 7 (1): 33–76. doi:10.1163/156853955X00021.
  24. ^ Baptista, L.F.; Lawson, R.; Visser, E.; Bell, D. A. (1999). "Relationships of some mannikins and waxbills in the estrildidae". Journal für Ornithologie. 140 (2): 179–192. Bibcode:1999JOrni.140..179B. doi:10.1007/BF01653597. S2CID 29184906.
  25. ^ Sikdar, M.; Kar, A. & Prakash, P. (1992). "Role of humidity in the seasonal reproduction of male spotted munia, Lonchura punctulata". Journal of Experimental Zoology. 264 (1): 82–84. doi:10.1002/jez.1402640112.
  26. ^ Gokula, V. (2001). "Nesting ecology of the Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in Mudumalai Wildlife Sanctuary (South India)". Acta Ornithologica. 36 (1): 1–5. doi:10.3161/068.036.0107. S2CID 84260813.
  27. ^ Sharma, R.C.; Bhatt, D. & Sharma, R.K. (2004). "Breeding success of the tropical Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in urbanized and forest habitats". Ornithological Science. 3 (2): 113–117. doi:10.2326/osj.3.113.
  28. ^ Lamba, B.S. (1974). "Nest construction technique of the Spotted Munia, Lonchura punctulata". Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society. 71 (3): 613–616.
  29. ^ Jump up to: a b Garrett, Garrett, John, Kimball (24 October 2016). "The Pin-Tailed Whydah as a Brood Parasite of the Scaly-Breasted Munia in Southern California" (PDF). Western Field Ornithologists.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  30. ^ Mehta, P. (1997). "Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata feeding on scat?". Newsletter for Birdwatchers. 37 (1): 16.
  31. ^ Nuijens, F.W.; Zweers, G.A. (1997). "Characters discriminating two seed husking mechanisms in finches (Fringillidae: Carduelinae) and estrildids (Passeridae: Estrildinae)". Journal of Morphology. 232 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4687(199704)232:1<1::AID-JMOR1>3.0.CO;2-G. PMID 29852621. S2CID 46921231.
  32. ^ Avery, M. L. (1980). "Diet and breeding seasonality among a population of sharp-tailed munias, Lonchura striata, in Malaysia" (PDF). The Auk. 97: 160–166. doi:10.1093/auk/97.1.160.
  33. ^ Stephens, D.W. (2007). A comprehensive guide to optimal foraging theory. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  34. ^ Pulliam, R.H. (1973). "On the advantages of flocking". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 38 (2): 419–422. Bibcode:1973JThBi..38..419P. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(73)90184-7. PMID 4734745.
  35. ^ Beauchamp, G. & Livoreil, B. (1997). "The effect of group size on vigilance and feeding rate in spice finches (Lonchura punctulata)". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 75 (9): 1526–1531. doi:10.1139/z97-776.
  36. ^ Giraldeau, L.A.; Beauchamp, G. (1999). "Food exploitation: searching for the optimal joining policy". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 14 (3): 102–106. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01542-0. PMID 10322509.
  37. ^ Rieucau, G. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2009). "Group size effect caused by food competition in nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata)". Behavioral Ecology. 20 (2): 421–425. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn144.
  38. ^ Gauvin, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2004). "Nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata) reduce their feeding rates in response to simulated competition". Oecologia. 139 (1): 150–156. Bibcode:2004Oecol.139..150G. doi:10.1007/s00442-003-1482-2. PMID 14722748. S2CID 21144047.
  39. ^ Jump up to: a b Giraldeau, L.-A. & Beauchamp, G. (1999). "Food exploitation: searching for the optimal joining policy". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 14 (3): 102–106. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01542-0. PMID 10322509.
  40. ^ Coolen, I.; Giraldeau, L.-A.; Lavoie, M. (2001). "Head position as an indication of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground-feeding bird". Animal Behaviour. 61 (5): 895–903. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1678. S2CID 53145727.
  41. ^ Coolen, I. (2002). «Увеличение размера группы кормления увеличивает использование Scrounger и снижает эффективность поиска в мускатном мускатном порядке ( Lonchura punctulata )». Поведенческая экология и социобиология . 52 (3): 232–238. doi : 10.1007/s00265-002-0500-4 . S2CID   28537757 .
  42. ^ Coolen, I. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2003). «Несовместимость между антипредиторной бдительности и тактикой Scrounger в мускатном мускатном порядке, Lonchura punctulata ». Поведение животных . 66 (4): 657–664. doi : 10.1006/anbe.2003.2236 . S2CID   53152430 .
  43. ^ Дэвис, Н. (2012). Введение в поведенческую экологию . Конкуренция за ресурсы: Wiley-Blackwell. С. 130–131. ISBN  978-1-4051-1416-5 .
  44. ^ Barnard, CJ & Subl, RM (1981). «Производители и схватки: общая модель и ее применение к пленным стадам домашних воробьев». Поведение животных . 29 (2): 543–550. doi : 10.1016/s0003-3472 (81) 80117-0 . S2CID   53170850 .
  45. ^ Beauchamp, G.; Giraldeau, L.-A. & Ennis, N. (1997). «Экспериментальные данные для поддержания специализаций на кормне по частоте зависимым от частоты выбора в стадах специй зябликов». ЭКОЛОГИЯ ИТОЛОГИЯ И ЭВОЛЮЦИЯ . 9 (2): 105–117. Bibcode : 1997etece ... 9..105b . doi : 10.1080/08927014.1997.9522890 .
  46. ^ Mottley, K. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2000). «Экспериментальные данные о том, что групповые фуражиры могут сходиться к прогнозируемым равновесиям производителя - Сккрунгера» (PDF) . Поведение животных . 60 (3): 341–350. doi : 10.1006/anbe.2000.1474 . PMID   11007643 . S2CID   35238033 .
  47. ^ Beauchamp, G. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1997). «Плач-эксплуатация в системе производителя-скрингера: тест гипотезы с использованием стада специй зябликов ( Lonchura punctulata . Поведенческая экология . 8 (1): 54–59. doi : 10.1093/beheco/8.1.54 .
  48. ^ Barrette, M. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2006). «Добыча криптичность уменьшает долю членов группы, ищущих еду». Поведение животных . 71 (5): 1183–1189. doi : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.008 . S2CID   53146661 .
  49. ^ Courant, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2008). «Конспецифическое присутствие делает эксплуатацию загадочной добычи более трудным в мускатном мускатном мускатном порядке». Поведение животных . 75 (3): 1101–1108. doi : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.023 . S2CID   54398287 .
  50. ^ Broom, M. & Ruxton, GD (1998). «Эволюционно стабильное воровство: теория игр применяется к клептопаразитизму» . Поведенческая экология . 9 (4): 397–403. doi : 10.1093/bheeco/9.4.397 .
  51. ^ Сиро, Э. (1999). «Эволюционно стабильная стратегия агрессивности в кормления группах» . Поведенческая экология . 11 (4): 351–356. doi : 10.1093/beheco/11.4.351 .
  52. ^ Дюбуа, Ф. (2002). «Защита ресурсов в контексте группы» . Поведенческая экология . 14 (1): 2–9. doi : 10.1093/beheco/14.1.2 .
  53. ^ Dubois, F. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2004). «Уменьшенная защита ресурсов в неопределенном мире: экспериментальный тест с использованием пленного мускатного ореха Mannikins». Поведение животных . 68 (1): 21–25. doi : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.025 . S2CID   54349286 .
  54. ^ «Торговля в диких птицах, уходящей в« неконтролируемой »во Вьетнаме: новый отчет» . Экологические новости Монгабая . 25 сентября 2017 года.
  55. ^ Бомфорд, М.; Синклер Р. (2002). «Австралийские исследования по птичьим вредителям: воздействие, управление и будущие направления». Эму . 102 (1): 29–45. Bibcode : 2002muao.102 ... 29b . doi : 10.1071/mu01028 . S2CID   83464835 .
  56. ^ Воротник, N.; Ньютон, я; Клемент, П. и Архипв, В. (2010). "Шеветка, зашнуной Мунией Лончур Пунклатат " В Del Hoyo, J.; Эллиотт, А. и Кристи, Д. (ред.). Птицы мира Тол. Том 15. Зяблики. Барселона: Последствия рыси. ISBN  978-84-96553-68-2 .
[ редактировать ]
Arc.Ask3.Ru: конец переведенного документа.
Arc.Ask3.Ru
Номер скриншота №: 0df47e26410bd62b513b0e718eca7372__1722950040
URL1:https://arc.ask3.ru/arc/aa/0d/72/0df47e26410bd62b513b0e718eca7372.html
Заголовок, (Title) документа по адресу, URL1:
Scaly-breasted munia - Wikipedia
Данный printscreen веб страницы (снимок веб страницы, скриншот веб страницы), визуально-программная копия документа расположенного по адресу URL1 и сохраненная в файл, имеет: квалифицированную, усовершенствованную (подтверждены: метки времени, валидность сертификата), открепленную ЭЦП (приложена к данному файлу), что может быть использовано для подтверждения содержания и факта существования документа в этот момент времени. Права на данный скриншот принадлежат администрации Ask3.ru, использование в качестве доказательства только с письменного разрешения правообладателя скриншота. Администрация Ask3.ru не несет ответственности за информацию размещенную на данном скриншоте. Права на прочие зарегистрированные элементы любого права, изображенные на снимках принадлежат их владельцам. Качество перевода предоставляется как есть. Любые претензии, иски не могут быть предъявлены. Если вы не согласны с любым пунктом перечисленным выше, вы не можете использовать данный сайт и информация размещенную на нем (сайте/странице), немедленно покиньте данный сайт. В случае нарушения любого пункта перечисленного выше, штраф 55! (Пятьдесят пять факториал, Денежную единицу (имеющую самостоятельную стоимость) можете выбрать самостоятельно, выплаичвается товарами в течение 7 дней с момента нарушения.)