Jump to content

Электрорецепция и электрогенез

Это хорошая статья. Нажмите здесь для получения дополнительной информации.

Слово -рыба представляет собой слабо электрическую морсридную рыбу, которая генерирует электрическое поле с его электрическим органом , а затем использует свои электрорецептивные нольенорганы и мормиромаст, чтобы найти близлежащие предметы по искажениям, которые они вызывают в электрическом поле. [ 1 ]

Электрорецепция и электрогенез являются тесно связанными биологическими способностями для восприятия электрических стимулов и генерации электрических полей . Оба используются для поиска добычи; Более сильные электрические разряды используются в нескольких группах рыб (наиболее известно, электрический угорь , который на самом деле не является угрем, а ножом ), чтобы ошеломить добычу. Возможности встречаются почти исключительно у водных или амфибийных животных, поскольку вода является гораздо лучшим проводником электроэнергии, чем воздух. При пассивной электрополокации такие объекты, как добыча, обнаруживаются путем измерения электрических полей, которые они создают. При активной электрополовации рыба генерирует слабое электрическое поле и ощущает различные искажения этого поля, созданного объектами, которые проводят или сопротивляются электричеству. Активная электрополокация практикуется двумя группами слабых электрических рыб , гимнообразными (ножами) и Mormyridae (слоновые рыбы) и Gymnarchus Niloticus , африканским ножом. Электрическая рыба генерирует электрическое поле, используя электрический орган , модифицирован из мышц в его хвосте. Поле называется слабым, если этого достаточно, только чтобы обнаружить добычу, и сильное, если оно достаточно мощно, чтобы оглушить или убить. Поле может быть в коротких импульсах, как в рыбах слонов или непрерывной волны, как в рыбах для нож. Некоторые сильно электрические рыбы, такие как электрический угорь , обнаруживают добычу, генерируя слабое электрическое поле, а затем сильно разряжают свои электрические органы, чтобы оглушить добычу; Другие сильно электрические рыбы, такие как электрический луч , пассивно -электрополаду. Звездные камеры уникальны в том, чтобы быть сильными электрическими, но не используя электрополокацию.

The electroreceptive ampullae of Lorenzini evolved early in the history of the vertebrates; they are found in both cartilaginous fishes such as sharks, and in bony fishes such as coelacanths and sturgeons, and must therefore be ancient. Most bony fishes have secondarily lost their ampullae of Lorenzini, but other non-homologous electroreceptors have repeatedly evolved, including in two groups of mammals, the monotremes (platypus and echidnas) and the cetaceans (Guiana dolphin).

History

[edit]
Hans Lissmann discovered electroreception in 1950 through his observations of Gymnarchus niloticus.[2]

In 1678, while doing dissections of sharks, the Italian physician Stefano Lorenzini discovered organs on their heads now called ampullae of Lorenzini. He published his findings in Osservazioni intorno alle torpedini.[3] The electroreceptive function of these organs was established by R. W. Murray in 1960.[4][5]

In 1921, the German anatomist Viktor Franz described the knollenorgans (tuberous organs) in the skin of the elephantfishes, again without knowledge of their function as electroreceptors.[6]

In 1949, the Ukrainian-British zoologist Hans Lissmann noticed that the African knife fish (Gymnarchus niloticus) was able to swim backwards at the same speed and with the same dexterity around obstacles as when it swam forwards, avoiding collisions. He demonstrated in 1950 that the fish was producing a variable electric field, and that the fish reacted to any change in the electric field around it.[2][7]

Electrolocation

[edit]
The electroreceptive ampullae of Lorenzini (red dots) evolved from the mechanosensory lateral line organs (gray lines) of early vertebrates.[8] They are seen here in the head of a shark.
Ampullae of Lorenzini, found in several basal groups of fishes, are jelly-filled canals connecting pores in the skin to sensory bulbs. They detect small differences in electrical potential between their two ends.

Electroreceptive animals use the sense to locate objects around them. This is important in ecological niches where the animal cannot depend on vision: for example in caves, in murky water, and at night. Electrolocation can be passive, sensing electric fields such as those generated by the muscle movements of buried prey, or active, the electrogenic predator generating a weak electric field to allow it to distinguish between conducting and non-conducting objects in its vicinity.[9]

Passive electrolocation

[edit]

In passive electrolocation, the animal senses the weak bioelectric fields generated by other animals and uses it to locate them. These electric fields are generated by all animals due to the activity of their nerves and muscles. A second source of electric fields in fish is the ion pump associated with osmoregulation at the gill membrane. This field is modulated by the opening and closing of the mouth and gill slits.[10][11] Passive electroreception usually relies upon ampullary receptors such as ampullae of Lorenzini which are sensitive to low frequency stimuli, below 50 Hz. These receptors have a jelly-filled canal leading from the sensory receptors to the skin surface.[8][9]

Active electrolocation

[edit]
A knollenorgan, a tuberous electroreceptor of weakly electric fish. RC=receptor cell; b.m.=basal membrane; n=nerve.
A Mormyromast, a type of electroreceptor found only in Mormyrid fishes

In active electrolocation,[12] the animal senses its surrounding environment by generating weak electric fields (electrogenesis) and detecting distortions in these fields using electroreceptor organs. This electric field is generated by means of a specialised electric organ consisting of modified muscle or nerves.[13] Animals that use active electroreception include the weakly electric fish, which either generate small electrical pulses (termed "pulse-type"), as in the Mormyridae, or produce a quasi-sinusoidal discharge from the electric organ (termed "wave-type"), as in the Gymnotidae.[14]

Many of these fish, such as Gymnarchus and Apteronotus, keep their body rather rigid, swimming forwards or backwards with equal facility by undulating fins that extend most of the length of their bodies. Swimming backwards may help them to search for and assess prey using electrosensory cues. Experiments by Lannoo and Lannoo in 1993 support Lissmann's proposal that this style of swimming with a straight back works effectively given the constraints of active electrolocation. Apteronotus can select and catch larger Daphnia water fleas among smaller ones, and they do not discriminate against artificially-darkened water fleas, in both cases with or without light.[7][15]

These fish create a potential usually smaller than one volt (1 V). Weakly electric fish can discriminate between objects with different resistance and capacitance values, which may help in identifying objects. Active electroreception typically has a range of about one body length, though objects with an electrical impedance similar to that of the surrounding water are nearly undetectable.[12][13][14]

Active electrolocation relies upon tuberous electroreceptors which are sensitive to high frequency (20-20,000 Hz) stimuli. These receptors have a loose plug of epithelial cells which capacitively couples the sensory receptor cells to the external environment. Elephantfish (Mormyridae) from Africa have tuberous electroreceptors known as Knollenorgans and Mormyromasts in their skin.[16][17][18]

Elephantfish emit short pulses to locate their prey. Capacitative and resistive objects affect the electric field differently, enabling the fish to locate objects of different types within a distance of about a body length. Resistive objects increase the amplitude of the pulse; capacitative objects introduce distortions.[1]

The Gymnotiformes, including the glass knifefish (Sternopygidae) and the electric eel (Gymnotidae), differ from the Mormyridae in emitting a continuous wave, approximating a sine wave, from their electric organ. As in the Mormyridae, the generated electric field enables them to discriminate accurately between capacitative and resistive objects.[1]

Electrolocation of capacitative and resistive objects in glass knifefish.
Many gymnotid fish generate a continuous electrical wave, which is
distorted differently by objects according to their conductivity.

The electric eel's electric organs occupy much of its body.
They can discharge both weakly for electrolocation
and strongly to stun prey.

Electrocommunication

[edit]
Electric eels create electric fields powerful enough to stun prey using modified muscles. Some weakly electric knifefishes appear to mimic the electric eel's discharge patterns; this may be Batesian mimicry, to deceive predators that they are too dangerous to attack.[20]

Weakly electric fish can communicate by modulating the electrical waveform they generate. They may use this to attract mates and in territorial displays.[21] Electric catfish frequently use their electric discharges to ward off other species from their shelter sites, whereas with their own species they have ritualized fights with open-mouth displays and sometimes bites, but rarely use electric organ discharges.[22]

When two glass knifefishes (Sternopygidae) come close together, both individuals shift their discharge frequencies in a jamming avoidance response.[13]

In bluntnose knifefishes, Brachyhypopomus, the electric discharge pattern is similar to the low voltage electrolocative discharge of the electric eel, Electrophorus. This is hypothesized to be Batesian mimicry of the powerfully-protected electric eel.[20] Brachyhypopomus males produce a continuous electric "hum" to attract females; this consumes 11–22% of their total energy budget, whereas female electrocommunication consumes only 3%. Large males produced signals of larger amplitude, and these are preferred by the females. The cost to males is reduced by a circadian rhythm, with more activity coinciding with night-time courtship and spawning, and less at other times.[23]

Fish that prey on electrolocating fish may "eavesdrop"[24] on the discharges of their prey to detect them. The electroreceptive African sharptooth catfish (Clarias gariepinus) may hunt the weakly electric mormyrid, Marcusenius macrolepidotus in this way.[25] This has driven the prey, in an evolutionary arms race, to develop more complex or higher frequency signals that are harder to detect.[26]

Some shark embryos and pups "freeze" when they detect the characteristic electric signal of their predators.[10]

Evolution and taxonomic distribution

[edit]

In vertebrates, passive electroreception is an ancestral trait, meaning that it was present in their last common ancestor.[27] The ancestral mechanism is called ampullary electroreception, from the name of the receptive organs involved, ampullae of Lorenzini. These evolved from the mechanical sensors of the lateral line, and exist in cartilaginous fishes (sharks, rays, and chimaeras), lungfishes, bichirs, coelacanths, sturgeons, paddlefishes, aquatic salamanders, and caecilians. Ampullae of Lorenzini appear to have been lost early in the evolution of bony fishes and tetrapods, though the evidence for absence in many groups is incomplete and unsatisfactory.[27] Where electroreception does occur in these groups, it has secondarily been acquired in evolution, using organs other than and not homologous with ampullae of Lorenzini.[8][27]

Electric organs have evolved at least eight separate times, each one forming a clade: twice during the evolution of cartilaginous fishes, creating the electric skates and rays, and six times during the evolution of the bony fishes.[28] Passively-electrolocating groups, including those that move their heads to direct their electroreceptors, are shown without symbols. Non-electrolocating species are not shown.[27] Actively electrolocating fish are marked with a small yellow lightning flash and their characteristic discharge waveforms.[29] Fish able to deliver electric shocks are marked with a red lightning flash .[27]

Vertebrates
Lampreys

End‑bud recep.
Jawed fishes
Cartilaginous fishes

Selachimorpha (sharks)

Batoidea

Torpediniformes (electric rays)

other rays

Rajidae (skates)

430 mya
Bony fishes
Lobe-finned fishes

Coelacanths

Lungfishes

Amphibians

(aquatic salamanders, caecilians; others: lost)

Mammals
Monotremes

(platypus, echidna)

glands in snout
Cetaceans

(Guiana dolphin)

vibrissal crypts 
(lost)
Ray-finned fishes
425 mya
Amp. of Lorenz.

Cartilaginous fish

[edit]

Sharks and rays (Elasmobranchii) rely on electrolocation using their ampullae of Lorenzini in the final stages of their attacks, as can be demonstrated by the robust feeding response elicited by electric fields similar to those of their prey. Sharks are the most electrically sensitive animals known, responding to direct current fields as low as 5 nV/cm.[30][31][32][33]

Bony fish

[edit]

Two groups of teleost fishes are weakly electric and actively electroreceptive: the Neotropical knifefishes (Gymnotiformes) and the African elephantfishes (Notopteroidei), enabling them to navigate and find food in turbid water.[34] The Gymnotiformes include the electric eel, which besides the group's use of low-voltage electrolocation, is able to generate high voltage electric shocks to stun its prey. Such powerful electrogenesis makes use of large electric organs modified from muscles. These consist of a stack of electrocytes, each capable of generating a small voltage; the voltages are effectively added together (in series) to provide a powerful electric organ discharge.[35][36]

Monotremes

[edit]
The platypus is a monotreme mammal that has secondarily acquired electroreception. Its receptors are arranged in stripes on its bill, giving it high sensitivity to the sides and below; it makes quick turns of its head as it swims to detect prey.[37][38]

The monotremes, including the semi-aquatic platypus and the terrestrial echidnas, are one of the only groups of mammals that have evolved electroreception. While the electroreceptors in fish and amphibians evolved from mechanosensory lateral line organs, those of monotremes are based on cutaneous glands innervated by trigeminal nerves. The electroreceptors of monotremes consist of free nerve endings located in the mucous glands of the snout. Among the monotremes, the platypus (Ornithorhynchus anatinus) has the most acute electric sense.[37][38] The platypus localises its prey using almost 40,000 electroreceptors arranged in front-to-back stripes along the bill.[34] The arrangement is highly directional, being most sensitive off to the sides and below. By making short quick head movements called saccades, platypuses accurately locate their prey. The platypus appears to use electroreception along with pressure sensors to determine the distance to prey from the delay between the arrival of electrical signals and pressure changes in water.[38]

The electroreceptive capabilities of the four species of echidna are much simpler. Long-beaked echidnas (genus Zaglossus) have some 2,000 receptors, while short-beaked echidnas (Tachyglossus aculeatus) have around 400, near the end of the snout.[34] This difference can be attributed to their habitat and feeding methods. Western long-beaked echidnas feed on earthworms in leaf litter in tropical forests, wet enough to conduct electrical signals well. Short-beaked echidnas feeds mainly on termites and ants, which live in nests in dry areas; the nest interiors are presumably humid enough for electroreception to work.[39] Experiments have shown that echidnas can be trained to respond to weak electric fields in water and moist soil. The electric sense of the echidna is hypothesised to be an evolutionary remnant from a platypus-like ancestor.[38]

Dolphins

[edit]

Dolphins have evolved electroreception in structures different from those of fish, amphibians and monotremes. The hairless vibrissal crypts on the rostrum of the Guiana dolphin (Sotalia guianensis), originally associated with mammalian whiskers, are capable of electroreception as low as 4.8 μV/cm, sufficient to detect small fish. This is comparable to the sensitivity of electroreceptors in the platypus.[40]

Bees

[edit]

Until recently, electroreception was known only in vertebrates. Recent research has shown that bees can detect the presence and pattern of a static charge on flowers.[41]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Jump up to: a b c d von der Emde, G. (15 May 1999). "Active electrolocation of objects in weakly electric fish". Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (10): 1205–1215. doi:10.1242/jeb.202.10.1205. PMID 10210662.
  2. ^ Jump up to: a b Alexander, R. McNeill (2006). "A new sense for muddy water". Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006 209: 200-201, doi: 10.1242/jeb.10.1242/jeb.02012 (2): 200–201. doi:10.1242/jeb.10.1242/jeb.02012. PMID 16391343.
  3. ^ Lorenzini, Stefano (1678). Osservazioni intorno alle torpedini. Florence, Italy: Per l'Onofri. doi:10.5962/bhl.title.6883. OCLC 2900213.
  4. ^ Murray, R. W. (September 1960). "Electrical sensitivity of the ampullae of Lorenzini". Nature. 187 (4741): 957. Bibcode:1960Natur.187..957M. doi:10.1038/187957a0. PMID 13727039.
  5. ^ Murray, R. W. (March 1962). "The response of the ampullae of Lorenzini of elasmobranchs to electrical stimulation". Journal of Experimental Biology. 39: 119–28. doi:10.1242/jeb.39.1.119. PMID 14477490.
  6. ^ Franz, Viktor (1921). "Zur mikroscopischen Anatomie der Mormyriden". Zoologisch Jahrbuch Abteilung für Anatomie und Ontogonie. 42: 91–148.
  7. ^ Jump up to: a b Lissmann, Hans. "Continuous Electrical Signals from the Tail of a Fish, Gymnarchus Niloticus Cuv", in: Nature, 167, 4240 (1951), pp. 201–202.
  8. ^ Jump up to: a b c King, Benedict; Hu, Yuzhi; Long, John A. (11 February 2018). "Electroreception in early vertebrates: survey, evidence and new information". Palaeontology. 61 (3): 325–358. Bibcode:2018Palgy..61..325K. doi:10.1111/pala.12346.
  9. ^ Jump up to: a b Crampton, William G. R. (5 February 2019). "Electroreception, electrogenesis and electric signal evolution". Journal of Fish Biology. 95 (1): 92–134. Bibcode:2019JFBio..95...92C. doi:10.1111/jfb.13922. PMID 30729523. S2CID 73442571.
  10. ^ Jump up to: a b Coplin, S. P.; Whitehead, D. (2004). "The functional roles of passive electroreception in non-electric fishes". Animal Biology. 54 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1163/157075604323010024.
  11. ^ Bodznick, D.; Montgomery, J. C.; Bradley, D. J. (1992). "Suppression of Common Mode Signals Within the Electrosensory System of the Little Skate Raja erinacea" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 171 (Pt 1): 107–125. doi:10.1242/jeb.171.1.107.
  12. ^ Jump up to: a b Albert, J. S.; Crampton, W. G. (2006). "Electroreception and Electrogenesis". In Lutz, P. L. (ed.). The Physiology of Fishes. Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. pp. 429–470. ISBN 978-0-8493-2022-4.
  13. ^ Jump up to: a b c Bullock, Theodore H.; Hamstra, R. Jr.; Scheich, H. (1972). "The jamming avoidance response of high frequency electric fish". Journal of Comparative Physiology (77): 1–22.
  14. ^ Jump up to: a b Babineau, D.; Longtin, A.; Lewis, J. E. (2006). "Modeling the Electric Field of Weakly Electric Fish". Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (Pt 18): 3636–3651. doi:10.1242/jeb.02403. PMID 16943504.
  15. ^ Lannoo, Michael J.; Lannoo, Susan Johnson (1993). "Why do electric fishes swim backwards? An hypothesis based on gymnotiform foraging behavior interpreted through sensory constraints". Environmental Biology of Fishes. 36 (2): 157–165. Bibcode:1993EnvBF..36..157L. doi:10.1007/bf00002795. S2CID 109426.
  16. ^ Bennett, M. V. L. (1965). "Electroreceptors in Mormyrids". Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. 30 (30): 245–262. doi:10.1101/SQB.1965.030.01.027. PMID 5219479.
  17. ^ Engelmann, Jacob; Nöbel, Sabine; Röver, Timo; von der Emde, Gerhard (2009). "The Schnauzenorgan-response of Gnathonemus petersii" (PDF). Frontiers in Zoology. 6 (21): 1–15. doi:10.1186/1742-9994-6-21. PMC 2760544. PMID 19772622.
  18. ^ Engelmann, Jacob; Bacelo, João; Metzen, Michael; Pusch, Roland; Bouton, Beatrice; Migliaro, Adriana; Caputi, Angel; Budelli, Ruben; Grant, Kirsty; von der Emde, Gerhard (20 May 2008). "Electric imaging through active electrolocation: implication for the analysis of complex scenes". Biological Cybernetics. 98 (6): 519–539. doi:10.1007/s00422-008-0213-5. PMID 18491164. S2CID 2975352.
  19. ^ Lewicki, M. S.; Olshausen, B. A.; Surlykke, A.; Moss, C. F. (2014). "Scene analysis in the natural environment". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 199. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00199. PMC 3978336. PMID 24744740.
  20. ^ Jump up to: a b Stoddard, P. K. (1999). "Predation enhances complexity in the evolution of electric fish signals". Nature. 400 (6741): 254–256. Bibcode:1999Natur.400..254S. doi:10.1038/22301. PMID 10421365. S2CID 204994529.
  21. ^ Hopkins, C. D. (1999). "Design features for electric communication". Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (Pt 10): 1217–1228. doi:10.1242/jeb.202.10.1217. PMID 10210663.
  22. ^ Rankin, Catharine H.; Moller, Peter (26 April 2010). "Social Behavior of the African Electric Catfish, Malapterurus electricus, during Intra- and Interspecific Encounters". Ethology. 73 (3). Wiley: 177–190. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00909.x.
  23. ^ Salazar, Vielka L.; Stoddard, Philip K. (15 March 2008). "Sex differences in energetic costs explain sexual dimorphism in the circadian rhythm modulation of the electrocommunication signal of the gymnotiform fish Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus". Journal of Experimental Biology. 211 (6): 1012–1020. doi:10.1242/jeb.014795. PMID 18310126. S2CID 14310938.
  24. ^ Falk, Jay J.; ter Hofstede, Hannah M.; Jones, Patricia L.; et al. (7 June 2015). "Sensory-based niche partitioning in a multiple predator–multiple prey community". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 282 (1808). doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.0520. PMC 4455811. PMID 25994677.
  25. ^ Merron, G. S. (1993). "Pack-hunting in two species of catfish, Clavias gariepinus and C. ngamensis, in the Okavango Delta, Botswana". Journal of Fish Biology. 43 (4): 575–584. Bibcode:1993JFBio..43..575M. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1993.tb00440.x.
  26. ^ Stoddard, P. K. (2002). "The evolutionary origins of electric signal complexity". Journal of Physiology – Paris. 96 (5–6): 485–491. doi:10.1016/S0928-4257(03)00004-4. PMID 14692496. S2CID 6240530.
  27. ^ Jump up to: a b c d e Bullock, Theodore H.; Bodznick, D. A.; Northcutt, R. G. (1983). "The phylogenetic distribution of electroreception: Evidence for convergent evolution of a primitive vertebrate sense modality" (PDF). Brain Research Reviews. 6 (1): 25–46. doi:10.1016/0165-0173(83)90003-6. hdl:2027.42/25137. PMID 6616267. S2CID 15603518.
  28. ^ Kirschbaum, Frank (2019). "Structure and Function of Electric Organs". The Histology of Fishes. Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. pp. 75–87. doi:10.1201/9780429113581-5. ISBN 978-0-429-11358-1. S2CID 216572032.
  29. ^ Kawasaki, M. (2011). "Detection and generation of electric signals". Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology. Elsevier. pp. 398–408. doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-374553-8.00136-2.
  30. ^ Fields, R. Douglas (August 2007). "The Shark's Electric Sense" (PDF). Scientific American. 297 (2): 74–80. Bibcode:2007SciAm.297b..74F. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0807-74. PMID 17894175. Retrieved 2 December 2013.
  31. ^ Lavoué, Sébastien; Miya, Masaki; Arnegard, Matthew E.; Sullivan, John P.; Hopkins, Carl D.; Nishida, Mutsumi (14 May 2012). "Comparable Ages for the Independent Origins of Electrogenesis in African and South American Weakly Electric Fishes". PLOS One. 7 (5): e36287. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...736287L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036287. PMC 3351409. PMID 22606250.
  32. ^ Kempster, R. M.; McCarthy, I. D.; Collin, S. P. (7 February 2012). "Phylogenetic and ecological factors influencing the number and distribution of electroreceptors in elasmobranchs". Journal of Fish Biology. 80 (5): 2055–2088. Bibcode:2012JFBio..80.2055K. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2011.03214.x. PMID 22497416.
  33. ^ Gardiner, Jayne M.; Atema, Jelle; Hueter, Robert E.; Motta, Philip J. (2 April 2014). "Multisensory Integration and Behavioral Plasticity in Sharks from Different Ecological Niches". PLOS One. 9 (4): e93036. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...993036G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0093036. PMC 3973673. PMID 24695492.
  34. ^ Jump up to: a b c "Electroreception in fish, amphibians and monotremes". Map of Life. 2010. Retrieved 12 June 2013.
  35. ^ Catania, Kenneth C. (October 2015). "Electric Eels Concentrate Their Electric Field to Induce Involuntary Fatigue in Struggling Prey". Current Biology. 25 (22): 2889–2898. Bibcode:2015CBio...25.2889C. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.036. PMID 26521183.
  36. ^ Froese, Rainer; Pauly, Daniel (eds.). "Electrophorus electricus". FishBase. December 2005 version.
  37. ^ Jump up to: a b Scheich, H.; Langner, G.; Tidemann, C.; Coles, R. B.; Guppy, A. (1986). "Electroreception and electrolocation in platypus". Nature. 319 (6052): 401–402. Bibcode:1986Natur.319..401S. doi:10.1038/319401a0. PMID 3945317. S2CID 4362785.
  38. ^ Jump up to: a b c d Pettigrew, J. D. (1999). "Electroreception in Monotremes" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (Pt 10): 1447–1454. doi:10.1242/jeb.202.10.1447. PMID 10210685.
  39. ^ Proske, U.; Грегори, JE; Игго А. (1998). «Сенсорные рецепторы в монотримах» . Философские транзакции Королевского общества б . 353 (1372): 1187–1198. doi : 10.1098/rstb.1998.0275 . PMC   1692308 . PMID   9720114 .
  40. ^ Чешский дамал, ню; Liebschner, A.; Miersch, L.; И др. (2012). «Электрорецепция в Duiana Dolphin ( Sotalia Guianensis . Труды Королевского общества б . 279 (1729): 663–668. Doi : 10.1098/rspb.2011.1127 . PMC   3248726 . PMID   21795271 .
  41. ^ Кларк, Д.; Уитни, Х.; Саттон, Г.; Роберт Д. (2013). «Обнаружение и изучение цветочных электрических полей от шмелей» . Наука . 340 (6128): 66–69. Bibcode : 2013sci ... 340 ... 66c . doi : 10.1126/science.1230883 . PMID   23429701 . S2CID   23742599 .

Дальнейшее чтение

[ редактировать ]
[ редактировать ]
Arc.Ask3.Ru: конец переведенного документа.
Arc.Ask3.Ru
Номер скриншота №: eb14dd6d20e86e11b5cf1cc6f1d72ab2__1724905320
URL1:https://arc.ask3.ru/arc/aa/eb/b2/eb14dd6d20e86e11b5cf1cc6f1d72ab2.html
Заголовок, (Title) документа по адресу, URL1:
Electroreception and electrogenesis - Wikipedia
Данный printscreen веб страницы (снимок веб страницы, скриншот веб страницы), визуально-программная копия документа расположенного по адресу URL1 и сохраненная в файл, имеет: квалифицированную, усовершенствованную (подтверждены: метки времени, валидность сертификата), открепленную ЭЦП (приложена к данному файлу), что может быть использовано для подтверждения содержания и факта существования документа в этот момент времени. Права на данный скриншот принадлежат администрации Ask3.ru, использование в качестве доказательства только с письменного разрешения правообладателя скриншота. Администрация Ask3.ru не несет ответственности за информацию размещенную на данном скриншоте. Права на прочие зарегистрированные элементы любого права, изображенные на снимках принадлежат их владельцам. Качество перевода предоставляется как есть. Любые претензии, иски не могут быть предъявлены. Если вы не согласны с любым пунктом перечисленным выше, вы не можете использовать данный сайт и информация размещенную на нем (сайте/странице), немедленно покиньте данный сайт. В случае нарушения любого пункта перечисленного выше, штраф 55! (Пятьдесят пять факториал, Денежную единицу (имеющую самостоятельную стоимость) можете выбрать самостоятельно, выплаичвается товарами в течение 7 дней с момента нарушения.)