Эволюционная психология
Часть серии о |
Психология |
---|
Эволюционная психология — это теоретический подход в психологии , который изучает познание и поведение с современной эволюционной точки зрения. [1] [2] человека Он направлен на выявление психологических адаптаций к наследственным проблемам, для решения которых они эволюционировали. С этой точки зрения психологические черты и механизмы являются либо функциональными продуктами естественного и полового отбора , либо неадаптивными побочными продуктами других адаптивных черт. [3] [4]
Адаптационистские взгляды на физиологические механизмы, такие как сердце, легкие и печень, распространены в эволюционной биологии . Эволюционные психологи применяют тот же подход в психологии, утверждая, что точно так же, как сердце развилось для перекачивания крови, печень — для детоксикации ядов, а почки — для фильтрации мутных жидкостей, существует модульность разума , заключающаяся в том, что разные психологические механизмы развились для решения различных адаптивных задач. проблемы. [5] Эти эволюционные психологи утверждают, что большая часть человеческого поведения является результатом психологических адаптаций , которые развились для решения повторяющихся проблем в среде предков человека. [6]
Some evolutionary psychologists argue that evolutionary theory can provide a foundational, metatheoretical framework that integrates the entire field of psychology in the same way evolutionary biology has for biology.[5][7][8]
Evolutionary psychologists hold that behaviors or traits that occur universally in all cultures are good candidates for evolutionary adaptations,[9] including the abilities to infer others' emotions, discern kin from non-kin, identify and prefer healthier mates, and cooperate with others. Findings have been made regarding human social behaviour related to infanticide, intelligence, marriage patterns, promiscuity, perception of beauty, bride price, and parental investment. The theories and findings of evolutionary psychology have applications in many fields, including economics, environment, health, law, management, psychiatry, politics, and literature.[10][11]
Criticism of evolutionary psychology involves questions of testability, cognitive and evolutionary assumptions (such as modular functioning of the brain, and large uncertainty about the ancestral environment), importance of non-genetic and non-adaptive explanations, as well as political and ethical issues due to interpretations of research results. Evolutionary psychologists frequently engage with and respond to such criticisms.[12][13][14]
Scope
[edit]Principles
[edit]Its central assumption is that the human brain is composed of a large number of specialized mechanisms that were shaped by natural selection over a vast periods of time to solve the recurrent information-processing problems faced by our ancestors. These problems involve food choices, social hierarchies, distributing resources to offspring, and selecting mates.[2] Proponents suggest that it seeks to integrate psychology into the other natural sciences, rooting it in the organizing theory of biology (evolutionary theory), and thus understanding psychology as a branch of biology. Anthropologist John Tooby and psychologist Leda Cosmides note:
Evolutionary psychology is the long-forestalled scientific attempt to assemble out of the disjointed, fragmentary, and mutually contradictory human disciplines a single, logically integrated research framework for the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences – a framework that not only incorporates the evolutionary sciences on a full and equal basis, but that systematically works out all of the revisions in existing belief and research practice that such a synthesis requires.[15]
Just as human physiology and evolutionary physiology have worked to identify physical adaptations of the body that represent "human physiological nature," the purpose of evolutionary psychology is to identify evolved emotional and cognitive adaptations that represent "human psychological nature." According to Steven Pinker, it is "not a single theory but a large set of hypotheses" and a term that "has also come to refer to a particular way of applying evolutionary theory to the mind, with an emphasis on adaptation, gene-level selection, and modularity." Evolutionary psychology adopts an understanding of the mind that is based on the computational theory of mind. It describes mental processes as computational operations, so that, for example, a fear response is described as arising from a neurological computation that inputs the perceptional data, e.g. a visual image of a spider, and outputs the appropriate reaction, e.g. fear of possibly dangerous animals. Under this view, any domain-general learning is impossible because of the combinatorial explosion. Evolutionary Psychology specifies the domain as the problems of survival and reproduction.[16]
While philosophers have generally considered the human mind to include broad faculties, such as reason and lust, evolutionary psychologists describe evolved psychological mechanisms as narrowly focused to deal with specific issues, such as catching cheaters or choosing mates. The discipline sees the human brain as having evolved specialized functions, called cognitive modules, or psychological adaptations which are shaped by natural selection.[17] Examples include language-acquisition modules, incest-avoidance mechanisms, cheater-detection mechanisms, intelligence and sex-specific mating preferences, foraging mechanisms, alliance-tracking mechanisms, agent-detection mechanisms, and others. Some mechanisms, termed domain-specific, deal with recurrent adaptive problems over the course of human evolutionary history. Domain-general mechanisms, on the other hand, are proposed to deal with evolutionary novelty.[18]
Evolutionary psychology has roots in cognitive psychology and evolutionary biology but also draws on behavioral ecology, artificial intelligence, genetics, ethology, anthropology, archaeology, biology, ecopsycology and zoology. It is closely linked to sociobiology,[9] but there are key differences between them including the emphasis on domain-specific rather than domain-general mechanisms, the relevance of measures of current fitness, the importance of mismatch theory, and psychology rather than behavior.
Nikolaas Tinbergen's four categories of questions can help to clarify the distinctions between several different, but complementary, types of explanations.[19] Evolutionary psychology focuses primarily on the "why?" questions, while traditional psychology focuses on the "how?" questions.[20]
Sequential vs. Static Perspective | |||
---|---|---|---|
Historical/Developmental Explanation of current form in terms of a historical sequence |
Current Form Explanation of the current form of species | ||
How vs. Why Questions | Proximate How an individual organism's structures function |
Ontogeny Developmental explanations for changes in individuals, from DNA to their current form |
Mechanism Mechanistic explanations for how an organism's structures work |
Evolutionary Why a species evolved the structures (adaptations) it has |
Phylogeny The history of the evolution of sequential changes in a species over many generations |
Adaptation A species trait that evolved to solve a reproductive or survival problem in the ancestral environment |
Premises
[edit]Evolutionary psychology is founded on several core premises.
- The brain is an information processing device, and it produces behavior in response to external and internal inputs.[5][21]
- The brain's adaptive mechanisms were shaped by natural and sexual selection.[5][21]
- Different neural mechanisms are specialized for solving problems in humanity's evolutionary past.[5][21]
- The brain has evolved specialized neural mechanisms that were designed for solving problems that recurred over deep evolutionary time,[21] giving modern humans stone-age minds.[5][22]
- Most contents and processes of the brain are unconscious; and most mental problems that seem easy to solve are actually extremely difficult problems that are solved unconsciously by complicated neural mechanisms.[5]
- Human psychology consists of many specialized mechanisms, each sensitive to different classes of information or inputs. These mechanisms combine to manifest behavior.[21]
History
[edit]Evolutionary psychology has its historical roots in Charles Darwin's theory of natural selection.[9] In The Origin of Species, Darwin predicted that psychology would develop an evolutionary basis:
In the distant future I see open fields for far more important researches. Psychology will be based on a new foundation, that of the necessary acquirement of each mental power and capacity by gradation.
— Darwin, Charles (1859). Wikisource.. p. 488 – via
Two of his later books were devoted to the study of animal emotions and psychology; The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex in 1871 and The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals in 1872. Darwin's work inspired William James's functionalist approach to psychology.[9] Darwin's theories of evolution, adaptation, and natural selection have provided insight into why brains function the way they do.[24]
The content of evolutionary psychology has derived from, on the one hand, the biological sciences (especially evolutionary theory as it relates to ancient human environments, the study of paleoanthropology and animal behavior) and, on the other, the human sciences, especially psychology.
Evolutionary biology as an academic discipline emerged with the modern synthesis in the 1930s and 1940s.[25] In the 1930s the study of animal behavior (ethology) emerged with the work of the Dutch biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen and the Austrian biologists Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch.
W.D. Hamilton's (1964) papers on inclusive fitness and Robert Trivers's (1972)[26] theories on reciprocity and parental investment helped to establish evolutionary thinking in psychology and the other social sciences. In 1975, Edward O. Wilson combined evolutionary theory with studies of animal and social behavior, building on the works of Lorenz and Tinbergen, in his book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis.
In the 1970s, two major branches developed from ethology. Firstly, the study of animal social behavior (including humans) generated sociobiology, defined by its pre-eminent proponent Edward O. Wilson in 1975 as "the systematic study of the biological basis of all social behavior"[27] and in 1978 as "the extension of population biology and evolutionary theory to social organization."[28] Secondly, there was behavioral ecology which placed less emphasis on social behavior; it focused on the ecological and evolutionary basis of animal and human behavior.
In the 1970s and 1980s university departments began to include the term evolutionary biology in their titles. The modern era of evolutionary psychology was ushered in, in particular, by Donald Symons' 1979 book The Evolution of Human Sexuality and Leda Cosmides and John Tooby's 1992 book The Adapted Mind.[9] David Buller observed that the term "evolutionary psychology" is sometimes seen as denoting research based on the specific methodological and theoretical commitments of certain researchers from the Santa Barbara school (University of California), thus some evolutionary psychologists prefer to term their work "human ecology", "human behavioural ecology" or "evolutionary anthropology" instead.[29]
From psychology there are the primary streams of developmental, social and cognitive psychology. Establishing some measure of the relative influence of genetics and environment on behavior has been at the core of behavioral genetics and its variants, notably studies at the molecular level that examine the relationship between genes, neurotransmitters and behavior. Dual inheritance theory (DIT), developed in the late 1970s and early 1980s, has a slightly different perspective by trying to explain how human behavior is a product of two different and interacting evolutionary processes: genetic evolution and cultural evolution. DIT is seen by some as a "middle-ground" between views that emphasize human universals versus those that emphasize cultural variation.[30]
Theoretical foundations
[edit]The theories on which evolutionary psychology is based originated with Charles Darwin's work, including his speculations about the evolutionary origins of social instincts in humans. Modern evolutionary psychology, however, is possible only because of advances in evolutionary theory in the 20th century.
Evolutionary psychologists say that natural selection has provided humans with many psychological adaptations, in much the same way that it generated humans' anatomical and physiological adaptations.[31] As with adaptations in general, psychological adaptations are said to be specialized for the environment in which an organism evolved, the environment of evolutionary adaptedness.[31][32] Sexual selection provides organisms with adaptations related to mating.[31] For male mammals, which have a relatively high maximal potential reproduction rate, sexual selection leads to adaptations that help them compete for females.[31] For female mammals, with a relatively low maximal potential reproduction rate, sexual selection leads to choosiness, which helps females select higher quality mates.[31] Charles Darwin described both natural selection and sexual selection, and he relied on group selection to explain the evolution of altruistic (self-sacrificing) behavior. But group selection was considered a weak explanation, because in any group the less altruistic individuals will be more likely to survive, and the group will become less self-sacrificing as a whole.
In 1964, the evolutionary biologist William D. Hamilton proposed inclusive fitness theory, emphasizing a gene-centered view of evolution. Hamilton noted that genes can increase the replication of copies of themselves into the next generation by influencing the organism's social traits in such a way that (statistically) results in helping the survival and reproduction of other copies of the same genes (most simply, identical copies in the organism's close relatives). According to Hamilton's rule, self-sacrificing behaviors (and the genes influencing them) can evolve if they typically help the organism's close relatives so much that it more than compensates for the individual animal's sacrifice. Inclusive fitness theory resolved the issue of how altruism can evolve. Other theories also help explain the evolution of altruistic behavior, including evolutionary game theory, tit-for-tat reciprocity, and generalized reciprocity. These theories help to explain the development of altruistic behavior, and account for hostility toward cheaters (individuals that take advantage of others' altruism).[33]
Several mid-level evolutionary theories inform evolutionary psychology. The r/K selection theory proposes that some species prosper by having many offspring, while others follow the strategy of having fewer offspring but investing much more in each one. Humans follow the second strategy. Parental investment theory explains how parents invest more or less in individual offspring based on how successful those offspring are likely to be, and thus how much they might improve the parents' inclusive fitness. According to the Trivers–Willard hypothesis, parents in good conditions tend to invest more in sons (who are best able to take advantage of good conditions), while parents in poor conditions tend to invest more in daughters (who are best able to have successful offspring even in poor conditions). According to life history theory, animals evolve life histories to match their environments, determining details such as age at first reproduction and number of offspring. Dual inheritance theory posits that genes and human culture have interacted, with genes affecting the development of culture, and culture, in turn, affecting human evolution on a genetic level, in a similar way to the Baldwin effect.
Evolved psychological mechanisms
[edit]Evolutionary psychology is based on the hypothesis that, just like hearts, lungs, livers, kidneys, and immune systems, cognition has a functional structure that has a genetic basis, and therefore has evolved by natural selection. Like other organs and tissues, this functional structure should be universally shared amongst a species and should solve important problems of survival and reproduction.
Evolutionary psychologists seek to understand psychological mechanisms by understanding the survival and reproductive functions they might have served over the course of evolutionary history.[34][page needed] These might include abilities to infer others' emotions, discern kin from non-kin, identify and prefer healthier mates, cooperate with others and follow leaders. Consistent with the theory of natural selection, evolutionary psychology sees humans as often in conflict with others, including mates and relatives. For instance, a mother may wish to wean her offspring from breastfeeding earlier than does her infant, which frees up the mother to invest in additional offspring.[33][35] Evolutionary psychology also recognizes the role of kin selection and reciprocity in evolving prosocial traits such as altruism.[33] Like chimpanzees and bonobos, humans have subtle and flexible social instincts, allowing them to form extended families, lifelong friendships, and political alliances.[33] In studies testing theoretical predictions, evolutionary psychologists have made modest findings on topics such as infanticide, intelligence, marriage patterns, promiscuity, perception of beauty, bride price and parental investment.[36]
Another example would be the evolved mechanism in depression. Clinical depression is maladaptive and should have evolutionary approaches so it can become adaptive. Over the centuries animals and humans have gone through hard times to stay alive, which made our fight or flight senses evolve tremendously. For instances, mammalians have separation anxiety from their guardians which causes distress and sends signals to their hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis, and emotional/behavioral changes. Going through these types of circumstances helps mammals cope with separation anxiety.[37]
Historical topics
[edit]Proponents of evolutionary psychology in the 1990s made some explorations in historical events, but the response from historical experts was highly negative and there has been little effort to continue that line of research. Historian Lynn Hunt says that the historians complained that the researchers:
have read the wrong studies, misinterpreted the results of experiments, or worse yet, turned to neuroscience looking for a universalizing, anti-representational and anti-intentional ontology to bolster their claims.[38]
Hunt states that "the few attempts to build up a subfield of psychohistory collapsed under the weight of its presuppositions." She concludes that, as of 2014, the "'iron curtain' between historians and psychology...remains standing."[39]
Products of evolution: adaptations, exaptations, byproducts, and random variation
[edit]Not all traits of organisms are evolutionary adaptations. As noted in the table below, traits may also be exaptations, byproducts of adaptations (sometimes called "spandrels"), or random variation between individuals.[40]
Psychological adaptations are hypothesized to be innate or relatively easy to learn and to manifest in cultures worldwide. For example, the ability of toddlers to learn a language with virtually no training is likely to be a psychological adaptation. On the other hand, ancestral humans did not read or write, thus today, learning to read and write requires extensive training, and presumably involves the repurposing of cognitive capacities that evolved in response to selection pressures unrelated to written language.[41] However, variations in manifest behavior can result from universal mechanisms interacting with different local environments. For example, Caucasians who move from a northern climate to the equator will have darker skin. The mechanisms regulating their pigmentation do not change; rather the input to those mechanisms change, resulting in different outputs.
Adaptation | Exaptation | Byproduct | Random variation | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Definition | Organismic trait designed to solve an ancestral problem(s). Shows complexity, special "design", functionality | Adaptation that has been "re-purposed" to solve a different adaptive problem. | Byproduct of an adaptive mechanism with no current or ancestral function | Random variations in an adaptation or byproduct |
Physiological example | Bones / Umbilical cord | Small bones of the inner ear | White color of bones / Belly button | Bumps on the skull, convex or concave belly button shape |
Psychological example | Toddlers' ability to learn to talk with minimal instruction | Voluntary attention | Ability to learn to read and write | Variations in verbal intelligence |
One of the tasks of evolutionary psychology is to identify which psychological traits are likely to be adaptations, byproducts or random variation. George C. Williams suggested that an "adaptation is a special and onerous concept that should only be used where it is really necessary."[42] As noted by Williams and others, adaptations can be identified by their improbable complexity, species universality, and adaptive functionality.
Obligate and facultative adaptations
[edit]A question that may be asked about an adaptation is whether it is generally obligate (relatively robust in the face of typical environmental variation) or facultative (sensitive to typical environmental variation).[43] The sweet taste of sugar and the pain of hitting one's knee against concrete are the result of fairly obligate psychological adaptations; typical environmental variability during development does not much affect their operation. By contrast, facultative adaptations are somewhat like "if-then" statements. For example, adult attachment style seems particularly sensitive to early childhood experiences. As adults, the propensity to develop close, trusting bonds with others is dependent on whether early childhood caregivers could be trusted to provide reliable assistance and attention.[citation needed] The adaptation for skin to tan is conditional to exposure to sunlight; this is an example of another facultative adaptation. When a psychological adaptation is facultative, evolutionary psychologists concern themselves with how developmental and environmental inputs influence the expression of the adaptation.
Cultural universals
[edit]Evolutionary psychologists hold that behaviors or traits that occur universally in all cultures are good candidates for evolutionary adaptations.[9] Cultural universals include behaviors related to language, cognition, social roles, gender roles, and technology.[44] Evolved psychological adaptations (such as the ability to learn a language) interact with cultural inputs to produce specific behaviors (e.g., the specific language learned).
Basic gender differences, such as greater eagerness for sex among men and greater coyness among women,[45] are explained as sexually dimorphic psychological adaptations that reflect the different reproductive strategies of males and females.[33][46]
Evolutionary psychologists contrast their approach to what they term the "standard social science model," according to which the mind is a general-purpose cognition device shaped almost entirely by culture.[47][48]
Environment of evolutionary adaptedness
[edit]Evolutionary psychology argues that to properly understand the functions of the brain, one must understand the properties of the environment in which the brain evolved. That environment is often referred to as the "environment of evolutionary adaptedness".[32]
The idea of an environment of evolutionary adaptedness was first explored as a part of attachment theory by John Bowlby.[49] This is the environment to which a particular evolved mechanism is adapted. More specifically, the environment of evolutionary adaptedness is defined as the set of historically recurring selection pressures that formed a given adaptation, as well as those aspects of the environment that were necessary for the proper development and functioning of the adaptation.
Humans, the genus Homo, appeared between 1.5 and 2.5 million years ago, a time that roughly coincides with the start of the Pleistocene 2.6 million years ago. Because the Pleistocene ended a mere 12,000 years ago, most human adaptations either newly evolved during the Pleistocene, or were maintained by stabilizing selection during the Pleistocene. Evolutionary psychology, therefore, proposes that the majority of human psychological mechanisms are adapted to reproductive problems frequently encountered in Pleistocene environments.[50] In broad terms, these problems include those of growth, development, differentiation, maintenance, mating, parenting, and social relationships.
The environment of evolutionary adaptedness is significantly different from modern society.[51] The ancestors of modern humans lived in smaller groups, had more cohesive cultures, and had more stable and rich contexts for identity and meaning.[51] Researchers look to existing hunter-gatherer societies for clues as to how hunter-gatherers lived in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness.[33] Unfortunately, the few surviving hunter-gatherer societies are different from each other, and they have been pushed out of the best land and into harsh environments, so it is not clear how closely they reflect ancestral culture.[33] However, all around the world small-band hunter-gatherers offer a similar developmental system for the young ("hunter-gatherer childhood model," Konner, 2005; "evolved developmental niche" or "evolved nest;" Narvaez et al., 2013). The characteristics of the niche are largely the same as for social mammals, who evolved over 30 million years ago: soothing perinatal experience, several years of on-request breastfeeding, nearly constant affection or physical proximity, responsiveness to need (mitigating offspring distress), self-directed play, and for humans, multiple responsive caregivers. Initial studies show the importance of these components in early life for positive child outcomes.[52][53]
Evolutionary psychologists sometimes look to chimpanzees, bonobos, and other great apes for insight into human ancestral behavior.[33]
Mismatches
[edit]Since an organism's adaptations were suited to its ancestral environment, a new and different environment can create a mismatch. Because humans are mostly adapted to Pleistocene environments, psychological mechanisms sometimes exhibit "mismatches" to the modern environment. One example is the fact that although about 10,000 people are killed with guns in the US annually,[54] whereas spiders and snakes kill only a handful, people nonetheless learn to fear spiders and snakes about as easily as they do a pointed gun, and more easily than an unpointed gun, rabbits or flowers.[55] A potential explanation is that spiders and snakes were a threat to human ancestors throughout the Pleistocene, whereas guns (and rabbits and flowers) were not. There is thus a mismatch between humans' evolved fear-learning psychology and the modern environment.[56][57]
This mismatch also shows up in the phenomena of the supernormal stimulus, a stimulus that elicits a response more strongly than the stimulus for which the response evolved. The term was coined by Niko Tinbergen to refer to non-human animal behavior, but psychologist Deirdre Barrett said that supernormal stimulation governs the behavior of humans as powerfully as that of other animals. She explained junk food as an exaggerated stimulus to cravings for salt, sugar, and fats,[58] and she says that television is an exaggeration of social cues of laughter, smiling faces and attention-grabbing action.[59] Magazine centerfolds and double cheeseburgers pull instincts intended for an environment of evolutionary adaptedness where breast development was a sign of health, youth and fertility in a prospective mate, and fat was a rare and vital nutrient.[60] The psychologist Mark van Vugt recently argued that modern organizational leadership is a mismatch.[61] His argument is that humans are not adapted to work in large, anonymous bureaucratic structures with formal hierarchies. The human mind still responds to personalized, charismatic leadership primarily in the context of informal, egalitarian settings. Hence the dissatisfaction and alienation that many employees experience. Salaries, bonuses and other privileges exploit instincts for relative status, which attract particularly males to senior executive positions.[62]
Research methods
[edit]Evolutionary theory is heuristic in that it may generate hypotheses that might not be developed from other theoretical approaches. One of the major goals of adaptationist research is to identify which organismic traits are likely to be adaptations, and which are byproducts or random variations. As noted earlier, adaptations are expected to show evidence of complexity, functionality, and species universality, while byproducts or random variation will not. In addition, adaptations are expected to manifest as proximate mechanisms that interact with the environment in either a generally obligate or facultative fashion (see above). Evolutionary psychologists are also interested in identifying these proximate mechanisms (sometimes termed "mental mechanisms" or "psychological adaptations") and what type of information they take as input, how they process that information, and their outputs.[43] Evolutionary developmental psychology, or "evo-devo," focuses on how adaptations may be activated at certain developmental times (e.g., losing baby teeth, adolescence, etc.) or how events during the development of an individual may alter life-history trajectories.
Evolutionary psychologists use several strategies to develop and test hypotheses about whether a psychological trait is likely to be an evolved adaptation. Buss (2011)[63] notes that these methods include:
Cross-cultural Consistency. Characteristics that have been demonstrated to be cross-cultural human universals such as smiling, crying, facial expressions are presumed to be evolved psychological adaptations. Several evolutionary psychologists have collected massive datasets from cultures around the world to assess cross-cultural universality.
Function to Form (or "problem to solution"). The fact that males, but not females, risk potential misidentification of genetic offspring (referred to as "paternity uncertainty") led evolutionary psychologists to hypothesize that, compared to females, male jealousy would be more focused on sexual, rather than emotional, infidelity.
Form to Function (reverse-engineering – or "solution to problem"). Morning sickness, and associated aversions to certain types of food, during pregnancy seemed to have the characteristics of an evolved adaptation (complexity and universality). Margie Profet hypothesized that the function was to avoid the ingestion of toxins during early pregnancy that could damage fetus (but which are otherwise likely to be harmless to healthy non-pregnant women).
Corresponding Neurological Modules. Evolutionary psychology and cognitive neuropsychology are mutually compatible – evolutionary psychology helps to identify psychological adaptations and their ultimate, evolutionary functions, while neuropsychology helps to identify the proximate manifestations of these adaptations.
Current Evolutionary Adaptiveness. In addition to evolutionary models that suggest evolution occurs across large spans of time, recent research has demonstrated that some evolutionary shifts can be fast and dramatic. Consequently, some evolutionary psychologists have focused on the impact of psychological traits in the current environment. Such research can be used to inform estimates of the prevalence of traits over time. Such work has been informative in studying evolutionary psychopathology.[64]
Evolutionary psychologists also use various sources of data for testing, including experiments, archaeological records, data from hunter-gatherer societies, observational studies, neuroscience data, self-reports and surveys, public records, and human products.[65] Recently, additional methods and tools have been introduced based on fictional scenarios,[66] mathematical models,[67] and multi-agent computer simulations.[68]
Main areas of research
[edit]Foundational areas of research in evolutionary psychology can be divided into broad categories of adaptive problems that arise from evolutionary theory itself: survival, mating, parenting, family and kinship, interactions with non-kin, and cultural evolution.
Survival and individual-level psychological adaptations
[edit]Problems of survival are clear targets for the evolution of physical and psychological adaptations. Major problems the ancestors of present-day humans faced included food selection and acquisition; territory selection and physical shelter; and avoiding predators and other environmental threats.[69]
Consciousness
[edit]Consciousness meets George Williams' criteria of species universality, complexity,[70] and functionality, and it is a trait that apparently increases fitness.[71]
In his paper "Evolution of consciousness," John Eccles argues that special anatomical and physical adaptations of the mammalian cerebral cortex gave rise to consciousness.[72] In contrast, others have argued that the recursive circuitry underwriting consciousness is much more primitive, having evolved initially in pre-mammalian species because it improves the capacity for interaction with both social and natural environments by providing an energy-saving "neutral" gear in an otherwise energy-expensive motor output machine.[73] Once in place, this recursive circuitry may well have provided a basis for the subsequent development of many of the functions that consciousness facilitates in higher organisms, as outlined by Bernard J. Baars.[74] Richard Dawkins suggested that humans evolved consciousness in order to make themselves the subjects of thought.[75] Daniel Povinelli suggests that large, tree-climbing apes evolved consciousness to take into account one's own mass when moving safely among tree branches.[75] Consistent with this hypothesis, Gordon Gallup found that chimpanzees and orangutans, but not little monkeys or terrestrial gorillas, demonstrated self-awareness in mirror tests.[75]
The concept of consciousness can refer to voluntary action, awareness, or wakefulness. However, even voluntary behavior involves unconscious mechanisms. Many cognitive processes take place in the cognitive unconscious, unavailable to conscious awareness. Some behaviors are conscious when learned but then become unconscious, seemingly automatic. Learning, especially implicitly learning a skill, can take place seemingly outside of consciousness. For example, plenty of people know how to turn right when they ride a bike, but very few can accurately explain how they actually do so.[75]
Evolutionary psychology approaches self-deception as an adaptation that can improve one's results in social exchanges.[75]
Sleep may have evolved to conserve energy when activity would be less fruitful or more dangerous, such as at night, and especially during the winter season.[75]
Sensation and perception
[edit]Many experts, such as Jerry Fodor, write that the purpose of perception is knowledge, but evolutionary psychologists hold that its primary purpose is to guide action.[76] For example, they say, depth perception seems to have evolved not to help us know the distances to other objects but rather to help us move around in space.[76] Evolutionary psychologists say that animals from fiddler crabs to humans use eyesight for collision avoidance, suggesting that vision is basically for directing action, not providing knowledge.[76]
Building and maintaining sense organs is metabolically expensive, so these organs evolve only when they improve an organism's fitness.[76] More than half the brain is devoted to processing sensory information, and the brain itself consumes roughly one-fourth of one's metabolic resources, so the senses must provide exceptional benefits to fitness.[76] Perception accurately mirrors the world; animals get useful, accurate information through their senses.[76]
Scientists who study perception and sensation have long understood the human senses as adaptations to their surrounding worlds.[76] Depth perception consists of processing over half a dozen visual cues, each of which is based on a regularity of the physical world.[76] Vision evolved to respond to the narrow range of electromagnetic energy that is plentiful and that does not pass through objects.[76] Sound waves go around corners and interact with obstacles, creating a complex pattern that includes useful information about the sources of and distances to objects.[76] Larger animals naturally make lower-pitched sounds as a consequence of their size.[76] The range over which an animal hears, on the other hand, is determined by adaptation. Homing pigeons, for example, can hear the very low-pitched sound (infrasound) that carries great distances, even though most smaller animals detect higher-pitched sounds.[76] Taste and smell respond to chemicals in the environment that are thought to have been significant for fitness in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness.[76] For example, salt and sugar were apparently both valuable to the human or pre-human inhabitants of the environment of evolutionary adaptedness, so present-day humans have an intrinsic hunger for salty and sweet tastes.[76] The sense of touch is actually many senses, including pressure, heat, cold, tickle, and pain.[76] Pain, while unpleasant, is adaptive.[76] An important adaptation for senses is range shifting, by which the organism becomes temporarily more or less sensitive to sensation.[76] For example, one's eyes automatically adjust to dim or bright ambient light.[76] Sensory abilities of different organisms often coevolve, as is the case with the hearing of echolocating bats and that of the moths that have evolved to respond to the sounds that the bats make.[76]
Evolutionary psychologists contend that perception demonstrates the principle of modularity, with specialized mechanisms handling particular perception tasks.[76] For example, people with damage to a particular part of the brain have the specific defect of not being able to recognize faces (prosopagnosia).[76] Evolutionary psychology suggests that this indicates a so-called face-reading module.[76]
Learning and facultative adaptations
[edit]In evolutionary psychology, learning is said to be accomplished through evolved capacities, specifically facultative adaptations.[77] Facultative adaptations express themselves differently depending on input from the environment.[77] Sometimes the input comes during development and helps shape that development.[77] For example, migrating birds learn to orient themselves by the stars during a critical period in their maturation.[77] Evolutionary psychologists believe that humans also learn language along an evolved program, also with critical periods.[77] The input can also come during daily tasks, helping the organism cope with changing environmental conditions.[77] For example, animals evolved Pavlovian conditioning in order to solve problems about causal relationships.[77] Animals accomplish learning tasks most easily when those tasks resemble problems that they faced in their evolutionary past, such as a rat learning where to find food or water.[77] Learning capacities sometimes demonstrate differences between the sexes.[77] In many animal species, for example, males can solve spatial problems faster and more accurately than females, due to the effects of male hormones during development.[77] The same might be true of humans.[77]
Emotion and motivation
[edit]Motivations direct and energize behavior, while emotions provide the affective component to motivation, positive or negative.[78] In the early 1970s, Paul Ekman and colleagues began a line of research which suggests that many emotions are universal.[78] He found evidence that humans share at least five basic emotions: fear, sadness, happiness, anger, and disgust.[78] Social emotions evidently evolved to motivate social behaviors that were adaptive in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness.[78] For example, spite seems to work against the individual but it can establish an individual's reputation as someone to be feared.[78] Shame and pride can motivate behaviors that help one maintain one's standing in a community, and self-esteem is one's estimate of one's status.[33][78] Motivation has a neurobiological basis in the reward system of the brain. Recently, it has been suggested that reward systems may evolve in such a way that there may be an inherent or unavoidable trade-off in the motivational system for activities of short versus long duration.[79]
Cognition
[edit]Cognition refers to internal representations of the world and internal information processing. From an evolutionary psychology perspective, cognition is not "general purpose", but uses heuristics, or strategies, that generally increase the likelihood of solving problems that the ancestors of present-day humans routinely faced. For example, present-day humans are far more likely to solve logic problems that involve detecting cheating (a common problem given humans' social nature) than the same logic problem put in purely abstract terms.[80] Since the ancestors of present-day humans did not encounter truly random events, present-day humans may be cognitively predisposed to incorrectly identify patterns in random sequences. "Gamblers' Fallacy" is one example of this. Gamblers may falsely believe that they have hit a "lucky streak" even when each outcome is actually random and independent of previous trials. Most people believe that if a fair coin has been flipped 9 times and Heads appears each time, that on the tenth flip, there is a greater than 50% chance of getting Tails.[78] Humans find it far easier to make diagnoses or predictions using frequency data than when the same information is presented as probabilities or percentages, presumably because the ancestors of present-day humans lived in relatively small tribes (usually with fewer than 150 people) where frequency information was more readily available.[78]
Personality
[edit]Evolutionary psychology is primarily interested in finding commonalities between people, or basic human psychological nature. From an evolutionary perspective, the fact that people have fundamental differences in personality traits initially presents something of a puzzle.[81] (Note: The field of behavioral genetics is concerned with statistically partitioning differences between people into genetic and environmental sources of variance. However, understanding the concept of heritability can be tricky – heritability refers only to the differences between people, never the degree to which the traits of an individual are due to environmental or genetic factors, since traits are always a complex interweaving of both.)
Personality traits are conceptualized by evolutionary psychologists as due to normal variation around an optimum, due to frequency-dependent selection (behavioral polymorphisms), or as facultative adaptations. Like variability in height, some personality traits may simply reflect inter-individual variability around a general optimum.[81] Or, personality traits may represent different genetically predisposed "behavioral morphs" – alternate behavioral strategies that depend on the frequency of competing behavioral strategies in the population. For example, if most of the population is generally trusting and gullible, the behavioral morph of being a "cheater" (or, in the extreme case, a sociopath) may be advantageous.[82] Finally, like many other psychological adaptations, personality traits may be facultative – sensitive to typical variations in the social environment, especially during early development. For example, later-born children are more likely than firstborns to be rebellious, less conscientious and more open to new experiences, which may be advantageous to them given their particular niche in family structure.[83]
Shared environmental influences do play a role in personality and are not always of less importance than genetic factors. However, shared environmental influences often decrease to near zero after adolescence but do not completely disappear.[84]
Language
[edit]According to Steven Pinker, who builds on the work by Noam Chomsky, the universal human ability to learn to talk between the ages of 1 – 4, basically without training, suggests that language acquisition is a distinctly human psychological adaptation (see, in particular, Pinker's The Language Instinct). Pinker and Bloom (1990) argue that language as a mental faculty shares many likenesses with the complex organs of the body which suggests that, like these organs, language has evolved as an adaptation, since this is the only known mechanism by which such complex organs can develop.[85]
Pinker follows Chomsky in arguing that the fact that children can learn any human language with no explicit instruction suggests that language, including most of grammar, is basically innate and that it only needs to be activated by interaction. Chomsky himself does not believe language to have evolved as an adaptation, but suggests that it likely evolved as a byproduct of some other adaptation, a so-called spandrel. But Pinker and Bloom argue that the organic nature of language strongly suggests that it has an adaptational origin.[86]
Evolutionary psychologists hold that the FOXP2 gene may well be associated with the evolution of human language.[87] In the 1980s, psycholinguist Myrna Gopnik identified a dominant gene that causes language impairment in the KE family of Britain.[87] This gene turned out to be a mutation of the FOXP2 gene.[87] Humans have a unique allele of this gene, which has otherwise been closely conserved through most of mammalian evolutionary history.[87] This unique allele seems to have first appeared between 100 and 200 thousand years ago, and it is now all but universal in humans.[87] However, the once-popular idea that FOXP2 is a 'grammar gene' or that it triggered the emergence of language in Homo sapiens is now widely discredited.[88]
Currently, several competing theories about the evolutionary origin of language coexist, none of them having achieved a general consensus.[89] Researchers of language acquisition in primates and humans such as Michael Tomasello and Talmy Givón, argue that the innatist framework has understated the role of imitation in learning and that it is not at all necessary to posit the existence of an innate grammar module to explain human language acquisition. Tomasello argues that studies of how children and primates actually acquire communicative skills suggest that humans learn complex behavior through experience, so that instead of a module specifically dedicated to language acquisition, language is acquired by the same cognitive mechanisms that are used to acquire all other kinds of socially transmitted behavior.[90]
On the issue of whether language is best seen as having evolved as an adaptation or as a spandrel, evolutionary biologist W. Tecumseh Fitch, following Stephen J. Gould, argues that it is unwarranted to assume that every aspect of language is an adaptation, or that language as a whole is an adaptation. He criticizes some strands of evolutionary psychology for suggesting a pan-adaptionist view of evolution, and dismisses Pinker and Bloom's question of whether "Language has evolved as an adaptation" as being misleading. He argues instead that from a biological viewpoint the evolutionary origins of language is best conceptualized as being the probable result of a convergence of many separate adaptations into a complex system.[91] A similar argument is made by Terrence Deacon who in The Symbolic Species argues that the different features of language have co-evolved with the evolution of the mind and that the ability to use symbolic communication is integrated in all other cognitive processes.[92]
If the theory that language could have evolved as a single adaptation is accepted, the question becomes which of its many functions has been the basis of adaptation. Several evolutionary hypotheses have been posited: that language evolved for the purpose of social grooming, that it evolved as a way to show mating potential or that it evolved to form social contracts. Evolutionary psychologists recognize that these theories are all speculative and that much more evidence is required to understand how language might have been selectively adapted.[93]
Mating
[edit]Given that sexual reproduction is the means by which genes are propagated into future generations, sexual selection plays a large role in human evolution. Human mating, then, is of interest to evolutionary psychologists who aim to investigate evolved mechanisms to attract and secure mates.[94] Several lines of research have stemmed from this interest, such as studies of mate selection[95][96][97] mate poaching,[98] mate retention,[99] mating preferences[100] and conflict between the sexes.[101]
In 1972 Robert Trivers published an influential paper[102] on sex differences that is now referred to as parental investment theory. The size differences of gametes (anisogamy) is the fundamental, defining difference between males (small gametes – sperm) and females (large gametes – ova). Trivers noted that anisogamy typically results in different levels of parental investment between the sexes, with females initially investing more. Trivers proposed that this difference in parental investment leads to the sexual selection of different reproductive strategies between the sexes and to sexual conflict. For example, he suggested that the sex that invests less in offspring will generally compete for access to the higher-investing sex to increase their inclusive fitness. Trivers posited that differential parental investment led to the evolution of sexual dimorphisms in mate choice, intra- and inter- sexual reproductive competition, and courtship displays. In mammals, including humans, females make a much larger parental investment than males (i.e. gestation followed by childbirth and lactation). Parental investment theory is a branch of life history theory.
Buss and Schmitt's (1993) Sexual Strategies Theory[103] proposed that, due to differential parental investment, humans have evolved sexually dimorphic adaptations related to "sexual accessibility, fertility assessment, commitment seeking and avoidance, immediate and enduring resource procurement, paternity certainty, assessment of mate value, and parental investment." Their Strategic Interference Theory[104] suggested that conflict between the sexes occurs when the preferred reproductive strategies of one sex interfere with those of the other sex, resulting in the activation of emotional responses such as anger or jealousy.
Women are generally more selective when choosing mates, especially under long-term mating conditions. However, under some circumstances, short term mating can provide benefits to women as well, such as fertility insurance, trading up to better genes, reducing the risk of inbreeding, and insurance protection of her offspring.[105]
Due to male paternity uncertainty, sex differences have been found in the domains of sexual jealousy.[106][107] Females generally react more adversely to emotional infidelity and males will react more to sexual infidelity. This particular pattern is predicted because the costs involved in mating for each sex are distinct. Women, on average, should prefer a mate who can offer resources (e.g., financial, commitment), thus, a woman risks losing such resources with a mate who commits emotional infidelity. Men, on the other hand, are never certain of the genetic paternity of their children because they do not bear the offspring themselves. This suggests that for men sexual infidelity would generally be more aversive than emotional infidelity because investing resources in another man's offspring does not lead to the propagation of their own genes.[108]
Another interesting line of research is that which examines women's mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle.[109][110] The theoretical underpinning of this research is that ancestral women would have evolved mechanisms to select mates with certain traits depending on their hormonal status. Known as the ovulatory shift hypothesis, the theory posits that, during the ovulatory phase of a woman's cycle (approximately days 10–15 of a woman's cycle),[111] a woman who mated with a male with high genetic quality would have been more likely, on average, to produce and bear a healthy offspring than a woman who mated with a male with low genetic quality. These putative preferences are predicted to be especially apparent for short-term mating domains because a potential male mate would only be offering genes to a potential offspring. This hypothesis allows researchers to examine whether women select mates who have characteristics that indicate high genetic quality during the high fertility phase of their ovulatory cycles. Indeed, studies have shown that women's preferences vary across the ovulatory cycle. In particular, Haselton and Miller (2006) showed that highly fertile women prefer creative but poor men as short-term mates. Creativity may be a proxy for good genes.[112] Research by Gangestad et al. (2004) indicates that highly fertile women prefer men who display social presence and intrasexual competition; these traits may act as cues that would help women predict which men may have, or would be able to acquire, resources.
Parenting
[edit]Reproduction is always costly for women, and can also be for men. Individuals are limited in the degree to which they can devote time and resources to producing and raising their young, and such expenditure may also be detrimental to their future condition, survival and further reproductive output. Parental investment is any parental expenditure (time, energy etc.) that benefits one offspring at a cost to parents' ability to invest in other components of fitness (Clutton-Brock 1991: 9; Trivers 1972). Components of fitness (Beatty 1992) include the well-being of existing offspring, parents' future reproduction, and inclusive fitness through aid to kin (Hamilton, 1964). Parental investment theory is a branch of life history theory.
The benefits of parental investment to the offspring are large and are associated with the effects on condition, growth, survival, and ultimately, on the reproductive success of the offspring. However, these benefits can come at the cost of the parent's ability to reproduce in the future e.g. through the increased risk of injury when defending offspring against predators, the loss of mating opportunities whilst rearing offspring, and an increase in the time to the next reproduction. Overall, parents are selected to maximize the difference between the benefits and the costs, and parental care will likely evolve when the benefits exceed the costs.
The Cinderella effect is an alleged high incidence of stepchildren being physically, emotionally or sexually abused, neglected, murdered, or otherwise mistreated at the hands of their stepparents at significantly higher rates than their genetic counterparts. It takes its name from the fairy tale character Cinderella, who in the story was cruelly mistreated by her stepmother and stepsisters.[113] Daly and Wilson (1996) noted: "Evolutionary thinking led to the discovery of the most important risk factor for child homicide – the presence of a stepparent. Parental efforts and investments are valuable resources, and selection favors those parental psyches that allocate effort effectively to promote fitness. The adaptive problems that challenge parental decision-making include both the accurate identification of one's offspring and the allocation of one's resources among them with sensitivity to their needs and abilities to convert parental investment into fitness increments…. Stepchildren were seldom or never so valuable to one's expected fitness as one's own offspring would be, and those parental psyches that were easily parasitized by just any appealing youngster must always have incurred a selective disadvantage"(Daly & Wilson, 1996, pp. 64–65). However, they note that not all stepparents will "want" to abuse their partner's children, or that genetic parenthood is any insurance against abuse. They see step parental care as primarily "mating effort" towards the genetic parent.[114]
Family and kin
[edit]Inclusive fitness is the sum of an organism's classical fitness (how many of its own offspring it produces and supports) and the number of equivalents of its own offspring it can add to the population by supporting others.[115] The first component is called classical fitness by Hamilton (1964).
From the gene's point of view, evolutionary success ultimately depends on leaving behind the maximum number of copies of itself in the population. Until 1964, it was generally believed that genes only achieved this by causing the individual to leave the maximum number of viable offspring. However, in 1964 W. D. Hamilton proved mathematically that, because close relatives of an organism share some identical genes, a gene can also increase its evolutionary success by promoting the reproduction and survival of these related or otherwise similar individuals. Hamilton concluded that this leads natural selection to favor organisms that would behave in ways that maximize their inclusive fitness. It is also true that natural selection favors behavior that maximizes personal fitness.
Hamilton's rule describes mathematically whether or not a gene for altruistic behavior will spread in a population:
where
- is the reproductive cost to the altruist,
- is the reproductive benefit to the recipient of the altruistic behavior, and
- is the probability, above the population average, of the individuals sharing an altruistic gene – commonly viewed as "degree of relatedness".
The concept serves to explain how natural selection can perpetuate altruism. If there is an "altruism gene" (or complex of genes) that influences an organism's behavior to be helpful and protective of relatives and their offspring, this behavior also increases the proportion of the altruism gene in the population, because relatives are likely to share genes with the altruist due to common descent. Altruists may also have some way to recognize altruistic behavior in unrelated individuals and be inclined to support them. As Dawkins points out in The Selfish Gene (Chapter 6) and The Extended Phenotype,[116] this must be distinguished from the green-beard effect.
Although it is generally true that humans tend to be more altruistic toward their kin than toward non-kin, the relevant proximate mechanisms that mediate this cooperation have been debated (see kin recognition), with some arguing that kin status is determined primarily via social and cultural factors (such as co-residence, maternal association of sibs, etc.),[117] while others have argued that kin recognition can also be mediated by biological factors such as facial resemblance and immunogenetic similarity of the major histocompatibility complex (MHC).[118] For a discussion of the interaction of these social and biological kin recognition factors see Lieberman, Tooby, and Cosmides (2007)[119] (PDF).
Whatever the proximate mechanisms of kin recognition there is substantial evidence that humans act generally more altruistically to close genetic kin compared to genetic non-kin.[120][121][122]
Interactions with non-kin / reciprocity
[edit]Although interactions with non-kin are generally less altruistic compared to those with kin, cooperation can be maintained with non-kin via mutually beneficial reciprocity as was proposed by Robert Trivers.[26] If there are repeated encounters between the same two players in an evolutionary game in which each of them can choose either to "cooperate" or "defect", then a strategy of mutual cooperation may be favored even if it pays each player, in the short term, to defect when the other cooperates. Direct reciprocity can lead to the evolution of cooperation only if the probability, w, of another encounter between the same two individuals exceeds the cost-to-benefit ratio of the altruistic act:
- w > c/b
Reciprocity can also be indirect if information about previous interactions is shared. Reputation allows evolution of cooperation by indirect reciprocity. Natural selection favors strategies that base the decision to help on the reputation of the recipient: studies show that people who are more helpful are more likely to receive help. The calculations of indirect reciprocity are complicated and only a tiny fraction of this universe has been uncovered, but again a simple rule has emerged.[123] Indirect reciprocity can only promote cooperation if the probability, q, of knowing someone's reputation exceeds the cost-to-benefit ratio of the altruistic act:
- q > c/b
One important problem with this explanation is that individuals may be able to evolve the capacity to obscure their reputation, reducing the probability, q, that it will be known.[124]
Trivers argues that friendship and various social emotions evolved in order to manage reciprocity.[125] Liking and disliking, he says, evolved to help present-day humans' ancestors form coalitions with others who reciprocated and to exclude those who did not reciprocate.[125] Moral indignation may have evolved to prevent one's altruism from being exploited by cheaters, and gratitude may have motivated present-day humans' ancestors to reciprocate appropriately after benefiting from others' altruism.[125] Likewise, present-day humans feel guilty when they fail to reciprocate.[125] These social motivations match what evolutionary psychologists expect to see in adaptations that evolved to maximize the benefits and minimize the drawbacks of reciprocity.[125]
Evolutionary psychologists say that humans have psychological adaptations that evolved specifically to help us identify nonreciprocators, commonly referred to as "cheaters."[125] In 1993, Robert Frank and his associates found that participants in a prisoner's dilemma scenario were often able to predict whether their partners would "cheat", based on a half-hour of unstructured social interaction.[125] In a 1996 experiment, for example, Linda Mealey and her colleagues found that people were better at remembering the faces of people when those faces were associated with stories about those individuals cheating (such as embezzling money from a church).[125]
Strong reciprocity (or "tribal reciprocity")
[edit]Humans may have an evolved set of psychological adaptations that predispose them to be more cooperative than otherwise would be expected with members of their tribal in-group, and, more nasty to members of tribal out groups. These adaptations may have been a consequence of tribal warfare.[126] Humans may also have predispositions for "altruistic punishment" – to punish in-group members who violate in-group rules, even when this altruistic behavior cannot be justified in terms of helping those you are related to (kin selection), cooperating with those who you will interact with again (direct reciprocity), or cooperating to better your reputation with others (indirect reciprocity).[127][128]
Evolutionary psychology and culture
[edit]Though evolutionary psychology has traditionally focused on individual-level behaviors, determined by species-typical psychological adaptations, considerable work has been done on how these adaptations shape and, ultimately govern, culture (Tooby and Cosmides, 1989).[129] Tooby and Cosmides (1989) argued that the mind consists of many domain-specific psychological adaptations, some of which may constrain what cultural material is learned or taught. As opposed to a domain-general cultural acquisition program, where an individual passively receives culturally-transmitted material from the group, Tooby and Cosmides (1989), among others, argue that: "the psyche evolved to generate adaptive rather than repetitive behavior, and hence critically analyzes the behavior of those surrounding it in highly structured and patterned ways, to be used as a rich (but by no means the only) source of information out of which to construct a 'private culture' or individually tailored adaptive system; in consequence, this system may or may not mirror the behavior of others in any given respect." (Tooby and Cosmides 1989).[129]
Biological explanations of human culture also brought criticism to evolutionary psychology: Evolutionary psychologists see the human psyche and physiology as a genetic product and assume that genes contain the information for the development and control of the organism and that this information is transmitted from one generation to the next via genes.[130] Evolutionary psychologists thereby see physical and psychological characteristics of humans as genetically programmed. Even then, when evolutionary psychologists acknowledge the influence of the environment on human development, they understand the environment only as an activator or trigger for the programmed developmental instructions encoded in genes.[130][131] Evolutionary psychologists, for example, believe that the human brain is made up of innate modules, each of which is specialised only for very specific tasks, e. g. an anxiety module. According to evolutionary psychologists, these modules are given before the organism actually develops and are then activated by some environmental event. Critics object that this view is reductionist and that cognitive specialisation only comes about through the interaction of humans with their real environment, rather than the environment of distant ancestors.[130][131] Interdisciplinary approaches are increasingly striving to mediate between these opposing points of view and to highlight that biological and cultural causes need not be antithetical in explaining human behaviour and even complex cultural achievements.[132]
In psychology sub-fields
[edit]Developmental psychology
[edit]According to Paul Baltes, the benefits granted by evolutionary selection decrease with age. Natural selection has not eliminated many harmful conditions and nonadaptive characteristics that appear among older adults, such as Alzheimer disease. If it were a disease that killed 20-year-olds instead of 70-year-olds this might have been a disease that natural selection could have eliminated ages ago. Thus, unaided by evolutionary pressures against nonadaptive conditions, modern humans suffer the aches, pains, and infirmities of aging and as the benefits of evolutionary selection decrease with age, the need for modern technological mediums against non-adaptive conditions increases.[133]
Social psychology
[edit]As humans are a highly social species, there are many adaptive problems associated with navigating the social world (e.g., maintaining allies, managing status hierarchies, interacting with outgroup members, coordinating social activities, collective decision-making). Researchers in the emerging field of evolutionary social psychology have made many discoveries pertaining to topics traditionally studied by social psychologists, including person perception, social cognition, attitudes, altruism, emotions, group dynamics, leadership, motivation, prejudice, intergroup relations, and cross-cultural differences.[134][135][136][137]
When endeavouring to solve a problem humans at an early age show determination while chimpanzees have no comparable facial expression. Researchers suspect the human determined expression evolved because when a human is determinedly working on a problem other people will frequently help.[138]
Abnormal psychology
[edit]Adaptationist hypotheses regarding the etiology of psychological disorders are often based on analogies between physiological and psychological dysfunctions,[139] as noted in the table below. Prominent theorists and evolutionary psychiatrists include Michael T. McGuire, Anthony Stevens, and Randolph M. Nesse. They, and others, suggest that mental disorders are due to the interactive effects of both nature and nurture, and often have multiple contributing causes.[20]
Causal mechanism of failure or malfunction of adaptation | Physiological Example | Hypothesized Psychological Example |
---|---|---|
Functioning adaptation (adaptive defense) | Fever / Vomiting (functional responses to infection or ingestion of toxins) |
Mild depression or anxiety (functional responses to mild loss or stress[142]/ reduction of social interactions to prevent infection by contagious pathogens)[143] |
By-product of an adaptation(s) | Intestinal gas (byproduct of digestion of fiber) |
Sexual fetishes (?) (possible byproduct of normal sexual arousal adaptations that have 'imprinted' on unusual objects or situations) |
Adaptations with multiple effects | Sickle cell disease (Gene that imparts malaria resistance, in homozygous form, causes sickle cell anemia) | Schizophrenia or bipolar disorder (May be side-effects of adaptations for high levels of creativity, perhaps dependent on alternate developmental trajectories) |
Malfunctioning adaptation | Allergies (over-reactive immunological responses) |
Autism (possible malfunctioning of theory of mind module) |
Frequency-dependent morphs | The two sexes / Different blood and immune system types | Personality disorders (may represent alternative behavioral strategies possibly dependent on its prevalence in the population) |
Mismatch between ancestral & current environments | Type 2 Diabetes (May be related to the abundance of sugary foods in the modern world) |
More frequent modern interaction with strangers (compared to family and close friends) may predispose greater incidence of depression & anxiety |
Tails of normal distribution (bell curve) | Dwarfism or gigantism | Extremities of the distribution of cognitive and personality traits (e.g., extremely introversion and extraversion, or intellectual giftedness and intellectual disability) |
Evolutionary psychologists have suggested that schizophrenia and bipolar disorder may reflect a side-effect of genes with fitness benefits, such as increased creativity.[144] (Some individuals with bipolar disorder are especially creative during their manic phases and the close relatives of people with schizophrenia have been found to be more likely to have creative professions.[144]) A 1994 report by the American Psychiatry Association found that people with schizophrenia at roughly the same rate in Western and non-Western cultures, and in industrialized and pastoral societies, suggesting that schizophrenia is not a disease of civilization nor an arbitrary social invention.[144] Sociopathy may represent an evolutionarily stable strategy, by which a small number of people who cheat on social contracts benefit in a society consisting mostly of non-sociopaths.[20] Mild depression may be an adaptive response to withdraw from, and re-evaluate, situations that have led to disadvantageous outcomes (the "analytical rumination hypothesis")[142] (see Evolutionary approaches to depression).
Trofimova reviewed the most consistent psychological and behavioural sex differences in psychological abilities and disabilities and linked them to the Geodakyan's evolutionary theory of sex (ETS).[145] She pointed out that a pattern of consistent sex differences in physical, verbal and social dis/abilities corresponds to the idea of the ETS considering sex dimorphism as a functional specialization of a species. Sex differentiation, according to the ETS, creates two partitions within a species, (1) conservational (females), and (2) variational (males). In females, superiority in verbal abilities, higher rule obedience, socialisation, empathy and agreeableness can be presented as a reflection of the systemic conservation function of the female sex. Male superiority is mostly noted in exploratory abilities - in risk- and sensation seeking, spacial orientation, physical strength and higher rates in physical aggression. In combination with higher birth and accidental death rates this pattern might be a reflection of the systemic variational function (testing the boundaries of beneficial characteristics) of the male sex. As a result, psychological sex differences might be influenced by a global tendency within a species to expand its norm of reaction, but at the same time to preserve the beneficial properties of the species. Moreover, Trofimova[145] suggested a "redundancy pruning" hypothesis as an upgrade of the ETS theory. She pointed out to higher rates of psychopathy, dyslexia, autism and schizophrenia in males, in comparison to females. She suggested that the variational function of the "male partition" might also provide irrelevance/redundancy pruning of an excess in a bank of beneficial characteristics of a species, with a continuing resistance to any changes from the norm-driven conservational partition of species. This might explain a contradictory allocation of a high drive for social status/power in the male sex with the their least (among two sexes) abilities for social interaction. The high rates of communicative disorders and psychopathy in males might facilitate their higher rates of disengagement from normative expectations and their insensitivity to social disapproval, when they deliberately do not follow social norms.
Some of these speculations have yet to be developed into fully testable hypotheses, and a great deal of research is required to confirm their validity.[146][147]
Antisocial and criminal behavior
[edit]Evolutionary psychology has been applied to explain criminal or otherwise immoral behavior as being adaptive or related to adaptive behaviors. Males are generally more aggressive than females, who are more selective of their partners because of the far greater effort they have to contribute to pregnancy and child-rearing. Males being more aggressive is hypothesized to stem from the more intense reproductive competition faced by them. Males of low status may be especially vulnerable to being childless. It may have been evolutionary advantageous to engage in highly risky and violently aggressive behavior to increase their status and therefore reproductive success. This may explain why males are generally involved in more crimes, and why low status and being unmarried are associated with criminality. Furthermore, competition over females is argued to have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood, which is theorized to explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.[148] Some sociologists have underlined differential exposure to androgens as the cause of these behaviors, notably Lee Ellis in his evolutionary neuroandrogenic (ENA) theory.[149]
Many conflicts that result in harm and death involve status, reputation, and seemingly trivial insults.[148] Steven Pinker in his book The Better Angels of Our Nature argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible deterrence against aggression. Therefore, it was important to be perceived as having a credible reputation for retaliation, resulting in humans developing instincts for revenge as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important.[148]
Rape is theorized to be a reproductive strategy that facilitates the propagation of the rapist's progeny. Such a strategy may be adopted by men who otherwise are unlikely to be appealing to women and therefore cannot form legitimate relationships, or by high-status men on socially vulnerable women who are unlikely to retaliate to increase their reproductive success even further.[150] The sociobiological theories of rape are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation, and objections to this theory are made on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds.
Psychology of religion
[edit]Adaptationist perspectives on religious belief suggest that, like all behavior, religious behaviors are a product of the human brain. As with all other organ functions, cognition's functional structure has been argued to have a genetic foundation, and is therefore subject to the effects of natural selection and sexual selection. Like other organs and tissues, this functional structure should be universally shared amongst humans and should have solved important problems of survival and reproduction in ancestral environments. However, evolutionary psychologists remain divided on whether religious belief is more likely a consequence of evolved psychological adaptations,[151][152] or a byproduct of other cognitive adaptations.[153]
Coalitional psychology
[edit]Coalitional psychology is an approach to explain political behaviors between different coalitions and the conditionality of these behaviors in evolutionary psychological perspective. This approach assumes that since human beings appeared on the earth, they have evolved to live in groups instead of living as individuals to achieve benefits such as more mating opportunities and increased status.[154] Human beings thus naturally think and act in a way that manages and negotiates group dynamics.
Coalitional psychology offers falsifiable ex ante prediction by positing five hypotheses on how these psychological adaptations operate:[155]
- Humans represent groups as a special category of individual, unstable and with a short shadow of the future
- Political entrepreneurs strategically manipulate the coalitional environment, often appealing to emotional devices such as "outrage" to inspire collective action.
- Relative gains dominate relations with enemies, whereas absolute gains characterize relations with allies.
- Coalitional size and male physical strength will positively predict individual support for aggressive foreign policies.
- Individuals with children, particularly women, will vary in adopting aggressive foreign policies than those without progeny.
Reception and criticism
[edit]Critics of evolutionary psychology accuse it of promoting genetic determinism, pan-adaptationism (the idea that all behaviors and anatomical features are adaptations), unfalsifiable hypotheses, distal or ultimate explanations of behavior when proximate explanations are superior, and malevolent political or moral ideas.[156]
Ethical implications
[edit]Critics have argued that evolutionary psychology might be used to justify existing social hierarchies and reactionary policies.[157][158] It has also been suggested by critics that evolutionary psychologists' theories and interpretations of empirical data rely heavily on ideological assumptions about race and gender.[159]
In response to such criticism, evolutionary psychologists often caution against committing the naturalistic fallacy – the assumption that "what is natural" is necessarily a moral good.[158][160][page needed][161] However, their caution against committing the naturalistic fallacy has been criticized as means to stifle legitimate ethical discussions.[158]
Contradictions in models
[edit]Some criticisms of evolutionary psychology point at contradictions between different aspects of adaptive scenarios posited by evolutionary psychology. One example is the evolutionary psychology model of extended social groups selecting for modern human brains, a contradiction being that the synaptic function of modern human brains require high amounts of many specific essential nutrients so that such a transition to higher requirements of the same essential nutrients being shared by all individuals in a population would decrease the possibility of forming large groups due to bottleneck foods with rare essential nutrients capping group sizes. It is mentioned that some insects have societies with different ranks for each individual and that monkeys remain socially functioning after the removal of most of the brain as additional arguments against big brains promoting social networking. The model of males as both providers and protectors is criticized for the impossibility of being in two places at once, the male cannot both protect his family at home and be out hunting at the same time. In the case of the claim that a provider male could buy protection service for his family from other males by bartering food that he had hunted, critics point at the fact that the most valuable food (the food that contained the rarest essential nutrients) would be different in different ecologies and as such vegetable in some geographical areas and animal in others, making it impossible for hunting styles relying on physical strength or risk-taking to be universally of similar value in bartered food and instead of making it inevitable that in some parts of Africa, food gathered with no need for major physical strength would be the most valuable to barter for protection. A contradiction between evolutionary psychology's claim of men needing to be more sexually visual than women for fast speed of assessing women's fertility than women needed to be able to assess the male's genes and its claim of male sexual jealousy guarding against infidelity is also pointed at, as it would be pointless for a male to be fast to assess female fertility if he needed to assess the risk of there being a jealous male mate and in that case his chances of defeating him before mating anyway (pointlessness of assessing one necessary condition faster than another necessary condition can possibly be assessed).[162][163]
Standard social science model
[edit]Evolutionary psychology has been entangled in the larger philosophical and social science controversies related to the debate on nature versus nurture. Evolutionary psychologists typically contrast evolutionary psychology with what they call the standard social science model (SSSM). They characterize the SSSM as the "blank slate", "relativist", "social constructionist", and "cultural determinist" perspective that they say dominated the social sciences throughout the 20th century and assumed that the mind was shaped almost entirely by culture.[160]
Critics have argued that evolutionary psychologists created a false dichotomy between their own view and the caricature of the SSSM.[164][165][166] Other critics regard the SSSM as a rhetorical device or a straw man[161][164][167] and suggest that the scientists whom evolutionary psychologists associate with the SSSM did not believe that the mind was a blank state devoid of any natural predispositions.[161]
Reductionism and determinism
[edit]Some critics view evolutionary psychology as a form of genetic reductionism and genetic determinism,[168][169] a common critique being that evolutionary psychology does not address the complexity of individual development and experience and fails to explain the influence of genes on behavior in individual cases.[48] Evolutionary psychologists respond that they are working within a nature-nurture interactionist framework that acknowledges that many psychological adaptations are facultative (sensitive to environmental variations during individual development). The discipline is generally not focused on proximate analyses of behavior, but rather its focus is on the study of distal/ultimate causality (the evolution of psychological adaptations). The field of behavioral genetics is focused on the study of the proximate influence of genes on behavior.[170]
Testability of hypotheses
[edit]A frequent critique of the discipline is that the hypotheses of evolutionary psychology are frequently arbitrary and difficult or impossible to adequately test, thus questioning its status as an actual scientific discipline, for example because many current traits probably evolved to serve different functions than they do now.[9][171] Thus because there are a potentially infinite number of alternative explanations for why a trait evolved, critics contend that it is impossible to determine the exact explanation.[172] While evolutionary psychology hypotheses are difficult to test, evolutionary psychologists assert that it is not impossible.[173] Part of the critique of the scientific base of evolutionary psychology includes a critique of the concept of the Environment of Evolutionary Adaptation (EEA). Some critics have argued that researchers know so little about the environment in which Homo sapiens evolved that explaining specific traits as an adaption to that environment becomes highly speculative.[174] Evolutionary psychologists respond that they do know many things about this environment, including the facts that present day humans' ancestors were hunter-gatherers, that they generally lived in small tribes, etc.[175] Edward Hagen argues that the human past environments were not radically different in the same sense as the Carboniferous or Jurassic periods and that the animal and plant taxa of the era were similar to those of the modern world, as was the geology and ecology. Hagen argues that few would deny that other organs evolved in the EEA (for example, lungs evolving in an oxygen rich atmosphere) yet critics question whether or not the brain's EEA is truly knowable, which he argues constitutes selective scepticism. Hagen also argues that most evolutionary psychology research is based on the fact that females can get pregnant and males cannot, which Hagen observes was also true in the EEA.[176][177]
Джон Алкок описывает это как «аргумент отсутствия машины времени», поскольку критики утверждают, что, поскольку невозможно вернуться во времени в ЕЭЗ, невозможно определить, что там происходило и, следовательно, что было адаптивным. Олкок утверждает, что современные данные позволяют исследователям быть достаточно уверенными в условиях ЕЭЗ и что тот факт, что так много человеческого поведения адаптивно в нынешней среде, является свидетельством того, что предковая среда людей имела много общего с нынешней. , поскольку это поведение развилось бы в среде предков. Таким образом, Олкок заключает, что исследователи могут делать прогнозы относительно адаптивной ценности черт. [178] Similarly, Dominic Murphy argues that alternative explanations cannot just be forwarded but instead need their own evidence and predictions - if one explanation makes predictions that the others cannot, it is reasonable to have confidence in that explanation. In addition, Murphy argues that other historical sciences also make predictions about modern phenomena to come up with explanations about past phenomena, for example, cosmologists look for evidence for what we would expect to see in the modern-day if the Big Bang was true, while geologists make predictions about modern phenomena to determine if an asteroid wiped out the dinosaurs. Murphy argues that if other historical disciplines can conduct tests without a time machine, then the onus is on the critics to show why evolutionary psychology is untestable if other historical disciplines are not, as "methods should be judged across the board, not singled out for ridicule in one context."[172]
Модульность ума
[ редактировать ]Эволюционные психологи обычно предполагают, что, как и тело, разум состоит из множества эволюционировавших модульных адаптаций. [179] хотя внутри дисциплины существуют некоторые разногласия относительно степени общей пластичности или «общности» некоторых модулей. [170] Было высказано предположение, что модульность развивается, потому что по сравнению с немодульными сетями она давала бы преимущество с точки зрения приспособленности. [180] и потому что затраты на подключение ниже. [181]
Напротив, некоторые ученые утверждают, что нет необходимости утверждать о существовании модулей, в значительной степени специфичных для предметной области, и предполагают, что нейронная анатомия мозга поддерживает модель, основанную на более общих способностях и процессах предметной области. [182] [183] Более того, эмпирическая поддержка предметно-ориентированной теории почти полностью основана на выполнении вариантов задачи выбора Уэйсона , объем которой чрезвычайно ограничен, поскольку она проверяет только один подтип дедуктивного рассуждения. [184] [185]
Культурное, а не генетическое развитие когнитивных инструментов
[ редактировать ]Психолог Сесилия Хейес утверждала, что картина человеческого разума, представленная некоторыми эволюционными психологами, представляет собой совокупность когнитивных инстинктов – органов мышления, сформированных генетической эволюцией в течение очень длительных периодов времени. [186] [22] – не соответствует результатам исследования. Вместо этого она утверждает, что у людей есть когнитивные гаджеты – «органы мышления специального назначения», созданные в ходе развития посредством социального взаимодействия. Аналогичную критику высказывает Субрена Э. Смит из Университета Нью-Гэмпшира . [187] [188] [189]
Ответ эволюционных психологов
[ редактировать ]Эволюционные психологи обращались ко многим своим критикам (например, в книгах Сегерстрале (2000), [190] Барков (2005), [191] [192] и Алкок (2001) [193] ). Среди их опровержений есть то, что некоторые критические замечания являются подставными лицами или основаны на неправильной дихотомии природы и воспитания или на фундаментальном непонимании дисциплины. [170] [194] [195] [196] [197] [198] [199]
Роберт Курзбан предположил, что «...критики поля, когда они ошибаются, не промахиваются. Их замешательство глубоко и глубоко. Это не то, чтобы они были стрелками, которые не могут точно попасть в центр мишени; они держишь пистолет задом наперед». [200] Многие написали специально для того, чтобы исправить основные заблуждения. [5] [13] [160] [201]
См. также
[ редактировать ]- Аффективная нейробиология
- Поведенческая генетика
- Биокультурная эволюция
- Биосоциальная криминология
- Коллективное бессознательное
- Когнитивная нейробиология
- Культурная нейробиология
- Дарвиновское счастье
- Дарвинистское литературоведение
- Глубокий социальный разум
- номер Данбара
- Эволюция мозга
- Список эволюционных психологов
- Эволюционное происхождение религий
- Эволюционная психология и культура
- Молекулярная эволюция
- Познание приматов
- Гоминидный интеллект
- Этология человека
- Язык великих обезьян
- Интеллект шимпанзе
- Гипотеза кооперативного глаза
- Ид, эго и суперэго
- Интерсубъективность
- Зеркальный нейрон
- Происхождение языка
- Происхождение речи
- Гипотеза овуляторного сдвига
- Сочувствие приматов
- Тень (психология)
- Теория моделирования эмпатии
- Теория разума
- Нейроэтология
- Палеолитическая диета
- Палеолитический образ жизни
- теория выбора r/K
- Социальная нейробиология
- Социобиология
- Универсальный дарвинизм
Примечания
[ редактировать ]- ^ Лонг, Жаклин Л. (11 мая 2016 г.). Психологическая энциклопедия Гейла (3-е изд.). Гейл Рисерч Инкорпорейтед. стр. 386–388. ISBN 978-1-4144-1204-7 . Проверено 10 июля 2022 г.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Галлахер, Микела. Б, Ирвинг (ред.). СПРАВОЧНИК ПО ПСИХОЛОГИИ . п. 1. ISBN 0471384089 .
- ^ Басс, Дэвид М. (2019). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме (6-е изд.). Нью-Йорк: Рутледж. п. 34. ISBN 978-1-138-08818-4 . OCLC 1084632387 .
- ^ Туби, Джон; Космидес, Леда (1 июля 1990 г.). «Прошлое объясняет настоящее: эмоциональная адаптация и структура среды обитания предков» . Этология и социобиология . 11 (4): 375–424. дои : 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z . ISSN 0162-3095 . S2CID 16405663 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час Космидес, Л .; Туби, Дж. (13 января 1997 г.). «Эволюционная психология: Букварь» . Центр эволюционной психологии. Архивировано из оригинала 24 июня 2020 года . Проверено 22 июля 2016 г.
- ^ Конфер и др. 2010 г.; Басс, 2005 г.; Даррант и Эллис, 2003 г.; Пинкер, 2002 г.; Туби и Космидес, 2005 г.
- ^ Дантли и Басс, 2008 г.
- ^ Кармен, РА и др. (2013). Эволюция интегрирована на всех островах поведенческого архипелага человека: вся психология как эволюционная психология. EvoS Journal: Журнал Консорциума эволюционных исследований, 5, стр. 108–26. ISSN 1944-1932 PDF
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г Шактер и др. 2007, стр. 26–27.
- ^ Оксфордский справочник по эволюционной психологии, под редакцией Робина Данбара и Луизы Баррет, Oxford University Press, 2007 г.
- ^ Справочник по эволюционной психологии под редакцией Дэвида М. Басса, John Wiley & Sons, 2005 г.
- ^ «Эволюционная психология: мощная предсказательная сила или пронизана банальными историями?» . Арео . 20 октября 2020 г. Проверено 23 декабря 2022 г.
- ^ Jump up to: а б « Да, но…» Ответы на десять распространённых критических замечаний в отношении эволюционной психологии – этот взгляд на жизнь» . 13 апреля 2015 года . Проверено 23 декабря 2022 г.
- ^ «Центр эволюционной психологии – Критический взгляд» . www.cep.ucsb.edu . Архивировано из оригинала 22 марта 2023 года . Проверено 23 декабря 2022 г.
- ^ Туби, Джон; Космидес, Леда (2005). «Концептуальный фундамент эволюционной психологии» . В Бассе, Дэвид М. (ред.). Справочник по эволюционной психологии . Джон Уайли и сыновья. п. 5. ISBN 9780470939376 . OCLC 61514485 . Проверено 19 июля 2021 г.
- ^ Басс, Дэвид М. «Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме», 5-е издание. страницы 28-29.
- ^ Басс, Дэвид. «Эволюционные теории в психологии» . Серия учебников НОБА . Издательство ДЕФ . Проверено 9 апреля 2021 г.
- ^ Чиаппе, Дэн; Макдональд, Кевин (2005). «Эволюция общих механизмов в интеллекте и обучении» . Журнал общей психологии . 132 (1): 5–40. дои : 10.3200/GENP.132.1.5-40 . ПМИД 15685958 . S2CID 6194752 . Проверено 9 апреля 2021 г.
- ^ Нессе, РМ (2000). Организованы четыре вопроса Тингергена. Читать онлайн. Архивировано 18 января 2012 года в Wayback Machine .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, стр. 1–24.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и «Лаборатория Басса – эволюционная психология в Техасском университете» . Проверено 10 августа 2016 г.
- ^ Jump up to: а б «Я не могу поверить, что это эволюционная психология!» . 7 марта 2016 г.
- ^ «Нобелевская премия по физиологии и медицине 1973 года» . Нобелевский фонд . Проверено 28 июля 2007 г.
- ^ Шахтер (10 декабря 2010 г.). Психология 2-е изд . Стоит издательства. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2 .
- ^ Стерельный, Ким. 2009. В Русе, Майкл и Трэвис, Джозеф (редакторы) Уилсон, Эдвард О. (Предисловие) Эволюция: первые четыре миллиарда лет. Издательство Гарвардского университета, Кембридж, Массачусетс. ISBN 978-0-674-03175-3 . п. 314.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Триверс, РЛ (1971). «Эволюция взаимного альтруизма». Ежеквартальный обзор биологии . 46 (1): 35–57. дои : 10.1086/406755 . JSTOR 2822435 . S2CID 19027999 .
- ^ Уилсон, Эдвард О. 1975. Социобиология: новый синтез . Архивировано 1 января 2010 года в издательстве Wayback Machine Harvard University Press, Кембридж, Массачусетс. ISBN 0-674-00089-7 стр. 4.
- ^ Уилсон, Эдвард О. 1978. О человеческой природе . Издательство Гарвардского университета, Кембридж, Массачусетс. пикселей
- ^ Буллер, Дэвид Дж. Адаптация разума: эволюционная психология и настойчивые поиски человеческой природы. МИТ пресс, 2006, стр.8.
- ^ Лаланд, Кевин Н. и Джиллиан Р. Браун. 2002. Смысл и нонсенс: эволюционные взгляды на поведение человека. Издательство Оксфордского университета, Оксфорд. стр. 287–319.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, стр. 25–56.
- ^ Jump up to: а б См. также «Среда эволюционной адаптации», вариация термина, используемого в экономике, например, в Рубин, Пол Х. (2003). «Народная экономика». Южный экономический журнал . 70 (1): 157–171. дои : 10.2307/1061637 . JSTOR 1061637 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час я Райт 1995 г.
- ^ Басс, Дэвид (2015). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме . Бока-Ратон, Флорида: Psychology Press, отпечаток Тейлора и Фрэнсиса. ISBN 9781317345725 . OCLC 1082202213 .
- ^ Райт, Роберт. «Моральное животное: почему мы такие, какие мы есть: новая наука эволюционной психологии» . Проверено 15 октября 2013 г.
- ^ Сломан, Л. (апрель 2003 г.). «Развитые механизмы депрессии: роль и взаимодействие привязанности и социального ранга при депрессии» . Журнал аффективных расстройств . 74 (2): 107–121. дои : 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00116-7 . ПМИД 12706512 .
- ^ Хант, Линн (2014). «Я и его история» . Американский исторический обзор . 119 (5): 1576–86. дои : 10.1093/ahr/119.5.1576 . цитата стр 1576.
- ^ Хант, «Я и его история». п. 1578.
- ^ Басс и др. 1998 год
- ^ Пинкер, Стивен. (1994) Языковой инстинкт
- ^ Джордж К. Уильямс, Адаптация и естественный отбор . п. 4.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Басс, DM (2011). Эволюционная психология.
- ^ Браун, Дональд Э. (1991) Человеческие универсалии. Нью-Йорк: МакГроу-Хилл.
- ^ Саймонс, Д. (1979). Эволюция сексуальности человека. Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета. Глава 6.
- ^ Пинкер 2002
- ^ Барков и др. 1992 год
- ^ Jump up to: а б "инстинкт." Британская энциклопедия Британская энциклопедия Интернет. Британская энциклопедия, 2011. Интернет. 18 февраля 2011 г. [2] .
- ^ Боулби, Джон (1969). Вложение . Нью-Йорк: Основные книги. ISBN 9780465097159 .
- ^ Саймонс, Дональд (1992). «О применении и неправильном использовании дарвинизма в изучении человеческого поведения» . Адаптированный разум: эволюционная психология и порождение культуры . Издательство Оксфордского университета. стр. 137–59 . ISBN 978-0-19-510107-2 .
- ^ Нарваес и др. 2013 .
- ^ Нарваес и др. 2012 .
- ^ «CDC PDF» (PDF) .
- ^ Оман, А.; Минека, С. (2001). «Страхи, фобии и готовность: к развитому модулю страха и обучения страху» (PDF) . Психологический обзор . 108 (3): 483–522. дои : 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483 . ПМИД 11488376 . Проверено 16 июня 2008 г.
- ^ Пинкер, Стив (1999). «Как работает разум». Анналы Нью-Йоркской академии наук . 882 (1). WW Нортон: 386–89. Бибкод : 1999NYASA.882..119P . дои : 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08538.x . ПМИД 10415890 . S2CID 222083447 .
- ^ Хаген, Э.Х.; Хаммерштейн, П. (2006). «Теория игр и эволюция человека: критика некоторых недавних интерпретаций экспериментальных игр». Теоретическая популяционная биология . 69 (3): 339–48. дои : 10.1016/j.tpb.2005.09.005 . ПМИД 16458945 .
- ^ Барретт, Дейдра. Талия: Эволюционная наука, стоящая за нашим кризисом веса и фитнеса (2007). Нью-Йорк: WW Нортон. стр. 31–51.
- ^ Барретт, Дейдра. Сверхъестественные стимулы: как первобытные побуждения превысили их эволюционную цель. Нью-Йорк: WW Нортон, 2010 г.
- ^ Хаген, Э.; Хаммерштейн, П. (2006). «Теория игр и эволюция человека: критика некоторых недавних интерпретаций экспериментальных игр». Теоретическая популяционная биология . 69 (3): 339–48. дои : 10.1016/j.tpb.2005.09.005 . ПМИД 16458945 .
- ^ Ван Вугт, Марк и Ахуджа, Анджана. Естественный отбор: эволюционная наука о лидерстве (2011). Нью-Йорк: Харпер Бизнес.
- ^ Ван Вугт, Марк; Роней, Ричард (2014). «Эволюционная психология лидерства». Обзор организационной психологии . 4 : 74–95. дои : 10.1177/2041386613493635 . S2CID 145773713 .
- ^ Басс, DM (2011). Эволюционная психология. Глава 2. Бостон: Пирсон/А и Б.
- ^ Джейкобсон, Северная Каролина (2016). Текущая эволюционная адаптивность психических расстройств: уровень рождаемости, качество отношений между родителями и детьми и психические расстройства. Журнал аномальной психологии
- ^ Басс, Дэвид (2004). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме . Бостон: ISBN Pearson Education, Inc. 978-0-205-48338-9 .
- ^ Эльдакар, Омар Тонси; Уилсон, Дэвид Слоан; О'Горман., Рик (2006). «Эмоции и действия, связанные с альтруистической помощью и наказанием» (PDF) . Эволюционная психология . 4 : 274–86. дои : 10.1177/147470490600400123 . S2CID 53991283 . Архивировано из оригинала 5 января 2009 года . Проверено 15 августа 2010 г.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: неподходящий URL ( ссылка ) - ^ Эльдакар, Омар Тонси; Уилсон, Дэвид Слоан (2008). «Эгоизм как альтруизм второго порядка» . Труды Национальной академии наук США . 105 (19): 6982–86. Бибкод : 2008PNAS..105.6982E . дои : 10.1073/pnas.0712173105 . ПМК 2383986 . ПМИД 18448681 .
- ^ Лима, Франсиско В.С.; Хаджибеганович, Тарик; Штауффер., Дитрих (2009). «Эволюция этноцентризма в ненаправленных и направленных сетях Барабаши-Альберта». Физика А: Статистическая механика и ее приложения . 388 (24): 4999–5004. arXiv : 0905.2672 . Бибкод : 2009PhyA..388.4999L . дои : 10.1016/j.physa.2009.08.029 . S2CID 18233740 .
- ^ Басс, DM (2011). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме
- ^ * Николс, С.; Грэнтэм, Т. (2000). «Адаптивная сложность и феноменальное сознание» (PDF) . Философия науки . 67 (4): 648–70. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.515.9722 . дои : 10.1086/392859 . JSTOR 188711 . S2CID 16484193 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 13 августа 2017 года . Проверено 28 октября 2017 г.
- ^ Фриман и Херрон. Эволюционный анализ. 2007. Пирсон Эдьюкейшн, Нью-Джерси.
- ^ Экклс, Дж. К. (1992). «Эволюция сознания» . Труды Национальной академии наук Соединенных Штатов Америки . 89 (16): 7320–24. Бибкод : 1992PNAS...89.7320E . дои : 10.1073/pnas.89.16.7320 . JSTOR 2360081 . ПМК 49701 . ПМИД 1502142 .
- ^ Питерс, Фредерик «Сознание как рекурсивное пространственно-временное самоопределение»
- ^ Баарс, Бернард Дж. Когнитивная теория сознания. 1993. Издательство Кембриджского университета.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, с. 101–21.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час я дж к л м н тот п д р с т в v Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, стр. 81–101.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час я дж к Гаулин и Макберни, 2003 г., глава 8.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, стр. 121–42.
- ^ Белке, ТВ; Гарланд, Т. младший (2007). «Краткая возможность побегать не действует в качестве подкрепления для мышей, выбранных для высокой ежедневной частоты бега на колесе» . Журнал экспериментального анализа поведения . 88 (2): 199–213. дои : 10.1901/jeab.2007.62-06 . ЧВК 1986434 . ПМИД 17970415 .
- ^ Гаулин и МакБерни, 2003, Глава 7.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Гаулин и Макберни, 2003 г., глава 9.
- ^ Мили, Линда (2010). «Социобиология социопатии: интегрированная эволюционная модель». Поведенческие и мозговые науки . 18 (3): 523–41. дои : 10.1017/S0140525X00039595 . S2CID 53956461 .
- ^ Саллоуэй, Ф. (1996). Рожденный бунтовать . Нью-Йорк: Пантеон. ISBN 9780679442325 .
- ^ Бушар, Ти Джей (2004). «Влияние генетики на психологические особенности человека. Исследование» (PDF) . Современные направления психологической науки . 13 (4): 148–51. дои : 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00295.x . S2CID 17398272 . Проверено 14 сентября 2014 г.
- ^ Пинкер, С.; Блум, П. (1990). «Естественный язык и естественный отбор». Поведенческие и мозговые науки . 13 (4): 707–27. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.116.4044 . дои : 10.1017/S0140525X00081061 . S2CID 6167614 .
- ^ Уоркман, Лэнс и Уилл Ридер (2004) Эволюционная психология: введение. Издательство Кембриджского университета, с. 259
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и Уоркман, Лэнс и Уилл Ридер (2008). Эволюционная психология: введение. 2-е изд. Издательство Кембриджского университета. Глава 10.
- ^ Диллер, К.К. и Р.Л. Канн, 2009. Доказательства против генетической революции в языке, произошедшей 50 000 лет назад. В Р. Боте и К. Найт (ред.), «Колыбель языка». Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета, стр. 135–49.
- ^ Workman & Reader 2008:277 «Существует ряд гипотез, предполагающих, что язык развился для выполнения социальных функций, таких как социальный уход (для объединения больших групп), заключение социальных контрактов (чтобы обеспечить моногамию и обеспечение мужчин) и использование языка для того, чтобы произвести впечатление на потенциальных партнеров. Хотя каждая из этих гипотез имеет свои достоинства, каждая из них по-прежнему является весьма спекулятивной и требует большего количества доказательств из различных областей исследований (таких как лингвистика и антропология)».
- ^ Уоркман, Лэнс и Уилл Ридер (2004) Эволюционная психология: введение. Издательство Кембриджского университета, с. 267
- ^ В. Текумсе Фитч (2010) Эволюция языка. Издательство Кембриджского университета, стр. 65–66.
- ^ Дикон, Терренс В. (1997) Символические виды: коэволюция языка и мозга. WW Нортон и Ко
- ^ Уоркман, Лэнс и Уилл Ридер (2004) Эволюционная психология: введение. Издательство Кембриджского университета, с. 277
- ^ Уилсон, Г. Д. Любовь и инстинкт. Лондон: Темпл Смит, 1981.
- ^ Басс 1994 г.
- ^ Басс и Барнс 1986
- ^ Ли, НП; Бейли, Дж. М.; Кенрик, DT; Линсенмайер, JAW (2002). «Необходимость и роскошь предпочтений партнера: проверка компромиссов» (PDF) . Журнал личности и социальной психологии . 82 (6): 947–55. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.1700 . дои : 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.947 . ПМИД 12051582 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 10 сентября 2008 года . Проверено 16 июля 2008 г.
- ^ Шмитт и Басс 2001 г.
- ^ Басс 1988.
- ^ Шакелфорд, Шмитт и Басс (2005) Универсальные аспекты предпочтений человека в отношении партнера; Личность и индивидуальные различия 39
- ^ Басс, Дэвид М. (2008). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме . Бостон, Массачусетс: Omegatype Typography, Inc., с. iv. ISBN 978-0-205-48338-9 .
- ^ Триверс, Р. (1972). Родительские инвестиции и половой отбор. В книге Б. Кэмпбелла (ред.), «Половой отбор и происхождение человека» . Чикаго: Олдин-Атертон.
- ^ Басс, ДМ; Шмитт, Д.П. (1993). «Теория сексуальных стратегий: эволюционный взгляд на спаривание людей». Психологический обзор . 100 (2): 204–32. дои : 10.1037/0033-295x.100.2.204 . ПМИД 8483982 .
- ^ Басс, DM (1989). «Конфликт между полами: стратегическое вмешательство и вызов гнева и расстройства». Журнал личности и социальной психологии . 56 (5): 735–47. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.3950 . дои : 10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.735 . ПМИД 2724067 .
- ^ Браун, Энтони, изд. (2 сентября 2000 г.). «Женщины, естественно, распутны» . Наблюдатель . Проверено 10 августа 2016 г. - через The Guardian.
- ^ Басс 1989
- ^ Басс и др. 1992 год
- ^ Калат, JW (2013). Биологическая психология (11-е изд.). Cengage Обучение. ISBN 9781111831004 .
- ^ Хазелтон, Миннесота; Миллер, Г. Ф. (2006). «Женская рождаемость на протяжении всего цикла увеличивает краткосрочную привлекательность творческого интеллекта» (PDF) . Человеческая природа . 17 (1): 50–73. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.411.6385 . дои : 10.1007/s12110-006-1020-0 . ПМИД 26181345 . S2CID 6625639 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 4 января 2006 года.
- ^ Гангестад, Юго-Запад; Симпсон, Дж.А.; Казинс, Эй Джей; Гарвер-Апгар, CE; Кристенсен, ПН (2004). «Предпочтения женщин в отношении мужских поведенческих проявлений меняются в зависимости от менструального цикла» (PDF) . Психологическая наука . 15 (3): 203–07. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.371.3266 . дои : 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x . ПМИД 15016293 . S2CID 9820539 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 16 февраля 2012 года . Проверено 16 июля 2008 г.
- ^ Уилкокс, Эй Джей; Дансон, Д.Б.; Вайнберг, ЧР ; Трасселл, Дж.; Бэрд, Д.Д. (2001). «Вероятность зачатия при одном половом акте: предоставление контрольных показателей для оценки посткоитальных контрацептивов» . Контрацепция . 63 (4): 211–15. дои : 10.1016/S0010-7824(01)00191-3 . ПМИД 11376648 .
- ^ Миллер, Г. Ф. (2000b) Брачный разум: как сексуальный выбор повлиял на эволюцию человеческой природы . Якорные книги: Нью-Йорк.
- ^ Дейли, Матин и Марго И. Уилсон. (1999)
- ^ Дейли и Уилсон, 1998 г.
- ^ «Эволюционная психология – инклюзивный фитнес» . Проверено 10 августа 2016 г.
- ^ Докинз, Ричард, «Расширенный фенотип», Oxford University Press, 1982 (Глава 9)
- ^ Уэст, Стюарт А.; Эль Муден, Клэр; Гарднер, Энди (2011). «Шестнадцать распространенных заблуждений об эволюции сотрудничества у людей» (PDF) . Эволюция и поведение человека . 32 (4): 231–62. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.188.3318 . doi : 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 12 августа 2017 года . Проверено 25 октября 2017 г.
- ^ Виллингер, Дж. (2012). «Социальная дискриминация по количественной оценке иммуногенетического сходства» . Труды Королевского общества B: Биологические науки . 279 (1746): 4368–4374. дои : 10.1098/rspb.2012.1279 . ПМЦ 3479794 . ПМИД 22951741 .
- ^ Либерман, Д.; Туби, Дж.; Космидес, Л. (февраль 2007 г.). «Архитектура обнаружения родственников человека» . Природа . 445 (7129): 727–31. Бибкод : 2007Natur.445..727L . дои : 10.1038/nature05510 . ПМК 3581061 . ПМИД 17301784 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б Басс, DM (2011). Эволюционная психология. Монтерей: Брукс-Коул.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Гаулин и Макберни (2004), Эволюционная психология, 2-е изд. Нью-Йорк: Прентис Холл
- ^ Workman & Reader (2008), Эволюционная психология, 2-е изд. Кембридж: Издательство Кембриджского университета
- ^ Новак, Массачусетс ; Зигмунд, К. (1998). «Эволюция косвенной взаимности путем оценки изображений». Природа . 393 (6685): 573–77. Бибкод : 1998Natur.393..573N . дои : 10.1038/31225 . ПМИД 9634232 . S2CID 4395576 .
- ^ Фаулер, Джеймс Х. (22 сентября 2005 г.). «Человеческое сотрудничество: решена проблема безбилетника второго порядка?» . Природа . 437 (7058): E8, обсуждение E8–9. Бибкод : 2005Natur.437E...8F . дои : 10.1038/nature04201 . ПМИД 16177738 . S2CID 4425399 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с д и ж г час Гаулин, Стивен Дж. К. и Дональд Х. Макберни. Эволюционная психология. Прентис Холл. 2003. ISBN 978-0-13-111529-3 , глава 14, стр. 323–52.
- ^ Боулз, С. (2009). «Влияли ли войны между предковыми группами охотников-собирателей на эволюцию социального поведения человека». Наука . 324 (5932): 1293–98. Бибкод : 2009Sci...324.1293B . дои : 10.1126/science.1168112 . ПМИД 19498163 . S2CID 33816122 .
- ^ Гинтис, Х. (2000). «Сильная взаимность и человеческая социальность». Журнал теоретической биологии . 206 (2): 169–79. Бибкод : 2000JThBi.206..169G . CiteSeerX 10.1.1.335.7226 . дои : 10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111 . ПМИД 10966755 . S2CID 9260305 .
- ^ Генрих, Дж.; Чудек, М. (2012). «Понимание исследовательской программы». Поведенческие и мозговые науки . 35 (1): 29–30. дои : 10.1017/S0140525X11001397 . ПМИД 22289319 . S2CID 39959479 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б Туби, Дж.; Космидес, Л. (1989). «Эволюционная психология и порождение культуры, часть I. Теоретические соображения». Этология и социобиология . 10 (1–3): 29–49. дои : 10.1016/0162-3095(89)90012-5 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Эволюционная психология: пример бедности генетического детерминизма . В Марке Х.В. Ван Регенмортеле и Дэвиде Л. Халле, « Перспективы и пределы редукционизма в биомедицинских науках» . John Wiley & Sons, Хобокен (Нью-Джерси) 2002, ISBN 0-471-49850-5, стр. 233-254.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Дэвид Дж. Буллер: Адаптация разума: эволюционная психология и настойчивые поиски человеческой природы . MIT Press, Кембридж, Массачусетс, 2005 г., ISBN 978-0-262-02579-9, стр. 134–135.
- ^ Нильс Зиталер : Противоречивые объяснительные подходы в антропологии и эволюционной психологии к феномену визуального искусства. В: Бенджамин П. Ланге, Саша Шварц: Человеческая психика между природой и культурой . Берлин, 2015, стр. 74–82.
- ^ Сантрок, В. Джон (2005). Актуальный подход к развитию продолжительности жизни (3-е изд.). Нью-Йорк, штат Нью-Йорк: МакГроу-Хилл. п. 62.
- ^ Нойберг, С.Л., Кенрик, Д.Т., и Шаллер, М. (2010). Эволюционная социальная психология. В С.Т. Фиске, Д.Т. Гилберте и Г. Линдзи (ред.), Справочник по социальной психологии (5-е издание, том 2, стр. 761–96). Нью-Йорк: Джон Уайли и сыновья.
- ^ Шаллер М., Симпсон Дж. А. и Кенрик Д. Т. (ред.) (2006). Эволюция и социальная психология. Нью-Йорк: Psychology Press.
- ^ Ван Вугт, Марк; Шаллер, Марк (2008). «Эволюционные подходы к групповой динамике: Введение». Групповая динамика: теория, исследования и практика . 12 : 1–6. дои : 10.1037/1089-2699.12.1.1 .
- ^ Ван Вугт, Марк и Камеда, Тацуя. Эволюция и группы. В главе 12 «Групповые процессы Дж. Левина» (2012). Нью-Йорк: Psychology Press.
- ^ «Люди развили «игровое лицо» как мольбу о помощи, как предполагает исследование» . Живая наука . 5 марта 2014 года . Проверено 10 августа 2016 г.
- ^ Нессе, Р ; Уильямс, Джордж К. (1996). Почему мы болеем . Нью-Йорк: Винтаж. (адаптационный взгляд на физиологические и психологические дисфункции)
- ^ Workman & Reader (2008), Эволюционная психология, 2-е изд. Кембридж: Издательство Кембриджского университета,
- ^ Космидес, Л.; Туби, Дж. (1999). «К эволюционной таксономии излечимых состояний». Журнал аномальной психологии . 108 (3): 453–64. дои : 10.1037/0021-843X.108.3.453 . ПМИД 10466269 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б Эндрюс, PW; Томсон, Дж. А. (июль 2009 г.). «Светлая сторона грусти: депрессия как адаптация к анализу сложных проблем» . Психол. Преподобный . 116 (3): 620–654. дои : 10.1037/a0016242 . ПМЦ 2734449 . ПМИД 19618990 .
- ^ Рэйсон, CL, Миллер, AN (2012). Эволюционное значение депрессии в молекулярной психиатрии патогенного хозяина (PATHOS-D) 1–23. PDF .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Гаулин и Макберни, 2003, стр. 239–56.
- ^ Jump up to: а б Трофимова, И. (2015). «Отражают ли психологические половые различия эволюционное бисексуальное разделение?». Американский журнал психологии . 128 (4): 485–514. дои : 10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.4.0485 . ПМИД 26721176 .
- ^ О'Коннелл, Х (2004). «Эволюционная теория в психиатрии и психологии» . Ирландский журнал психологической медицины . 21 (1): 37. дои : 10.1017/s0790966700008193 . ПМИД 30308732 .
- ^ Роуз, С. (2001). «Возвращение к эволюционной психологии и психиатрии» . Британский журнал психиатрии . 179 (6): 558–59. дои : 10.1192/bjp.179.6.558-b . ПМИД 11731363 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Аурелио Хосе Фигередо, Пол Роберт Глэдден, Закари Хохман. Эволюционная психология преступного поведения. В Робертс, Южная Каролина (2011). Робертс, С. Крейг (ред.). Прикладная эволюционная психология . Издательство Оксфордского университета. doi : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 . ISBN 9780199586073 .
- ^ Эллис, Ли (2005). «Теория, объясняющая биологические корреляты преступности». Европейский журнал криминологии . 2 (3): 287–315. дои : 10.1177/1477370805054098 . ISSN 1477-3708 . S2CID 53587552 .
- ^ Хаген, Эдвард Х. «Часто задаваемые вопросы по эволюционной психологии» . anth.ucsb.edu . Проверено 16 мая 2016 г.
- ^ Сосис, Р.; Алькорта, К. (2003). «Сигнализация, солидарность и священное: эволюция религиозного поведения». Эволюционная антропология . 12 (6): 264–74. дои : 10.1002/evan.10120 . S2CID 443130 .
- ^ Шоцик К., Ван Эйген Х (2021). Пересмотр когнитивной и эволюционной науки о религии: Религия как адаптация . Чам: Спрингер. стр. 49–81. ISBN 9783030635152 .
- ^ Линард, П.; Бойер, П. (2006). «Откуда коллективные ритуалы? Модель культурного отбора ритуализованного поведения». Американский антрополог . 108 (4): 824–27. дои : 10.1525/aa.2006.108.4.814 .
- ^ Лопес, Энтони С.; Макдермотт, Роуз; Банг Петерсен, Майкл (2011). «Государства в уме: эволюция, коалиционная психология и международная политика». Международная безопасность . 36 (2): 61–66. дои : 10.1162/isec_a_00056 . S2CID 57562816 .
- ^ Лопес, Энтони С.; Макдермотт, Роуз; Банг Петерсен, Майкл (2011). «Государства в уме: эволюция, коалиционная психология и международная политика». Международная безопасность . 36 (2): 66–82. дои : 10.1162/isec_a_00056 . S2CID 57562816 .
- ^ Курцбан, Роберт. Увы, бедная эволюционная психология . Обзор человеческой природы, 2002 г., том 2: 99–109 (14 марта). Проверено 14 июля 2013 г.
- ^ Роуз, Хилари; Роуз, Стивен (2000). "Введение" . Увы, бедный Дарвин: аргументы против эволюционной психологии . Нью-Йорк: Книги Гармонии. стр. 1–13 . ISBN 978-0-609-60513-4 .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Уилсон, Дэвид Слоан; Дитрих, Эрик; Кларк, Энн Б. (2003). «О неуместном использовании натуралистической ошибки в эволюционной психологии» (PDF) . Биология и философия . 18 (5): 669–81. дои : 10.1023/А:1026380825208 . S2CID 30891026 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 15 апреля 2015 года . Проверено 23 марта 2013 г.
- ^ Капораэль, Линда Р.; Брюэр, Мэрилин Б. (1991). «В поисках человеческой природы: социальные и научные проблемы эволюционной психологии». Журнал социальных проблем . 47 (3): 1–9. дои : 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1991.tb01819.x .
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Пинкер, С. (2003). Чистый лист . Нью-Йорк: Пингвин
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Леви, Нил (2004). «Эволюционная психология, человеческие универсалии и стандартная модель социальных наук». Биология и философия . 19 (3): 459–72. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.90.9290 . дои : 10.1023/B:BIPH.0000036111.64561.63 . S2CID 10126372 .
- ^ WE Frankenhuis Экологическая непредсказуемость, Энциклопедия эволюционной психологической науки, 2016 - Springer
- ^ Дуглас Т. Кенрик, Виадас Гришкевичюс, Омар Махмуд Рациональное животное: как эволюция сделала нас умнее, чем мы думаем, 2016
- ^ Jump up to: а б Ричардсон, Роберт С. (2007). Эволюционная психология как неадаптированная психология . Кембридж, Массачусетс: MIT Press . п. 176. ИСБН 978-0-262-18260-7 .
- ^ Уоллес, Брендан (2010). Неправильное понимание Дарвина: почему эволюционная психология не работает . Эксетер: Выходные данные Академический. п. 136. ИСБН 978-1-84540-207-5 .
- ^ Соломон, Шелдон; и др. (2004). «Человеческое осознание смертности и эволюция культуры» . В Шаллере, Марк; Крэндалл, Кристиан С. (ред.). Психологические основы культуры . Махва, Нью-Джерси: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. п. 17. ISBN 978-0-8058-3839-8 .
- ^ Сэмпсон, Джеффри (2009). Дебаты о «языковом инстинкте»: исправленное издание . Лондон: Континуум. стр. 134–45. ISBN 978-0-8264-7384-4 .
- ^ Майерс, Вольфганг (2003). «Фальшивое утверждение эволюционной психологии» . В Стивенсоне, Ниам (ред.). Теоретическая психология: критический вклад . Конкорд, Онтарио: Публикации Университета Каптуса. стр. 426–35 . ISBN 978-1-55322-055-8 .
- ^ Плоткин, Генри. 2004 Эволюционная мысль в журнале «Психология: краткая история». Блэквелл. п. 150.
- ^ Jump up to: а б с Конфер, JC; Истон, Дж.А.; Флейшман, Д.С.; Гетц, CD; Льюис, DMG; Периллу, К.; Басс, DM (2010). «Эволюционная психология: противоречия, вопросы, перспективы и ограничения» (PDF) . Американский психолог . 65 (2): 110–26. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.601.8691 . дои : 10.1037/a0018413 . ПМИД 20141266 .
- ^ Райл, Энтони (2005). «Актуальность эволюционной психологии для психотерапии». Британский журнал психотерапии . 21 (3): 375–88. дои : 10.1111/j.1752-0118.2005.tb00225.x .
- ^ Jump up to: а б Мерфи, Доминик. «Адаптационизм и психологическое объяснение». В эволюционной психологии, стр. 161–184. Спрингер, Бостон, Массачусетс, 2003 г.
- ^ «Проверка идей об эволюционном происхождении психологических явлений действительно является сложной задачей, но не невозможной» (Buss et al. 1998; Pinker, 1997b).
- ^ Плоткин, Генри. 2004 Эволюционная мысль в журнале «Психология: краткая история». Блэквелл. п. 149.
- ^ Справочник по эволюционной психологии (2005), Дэвид М. Басс, Глава 1, стр. 5–67, Концептуальные основы эволюционной психологии, Джон Туби и Леда Космидес.
- ^ Хаген, Эдвард Х. Инвариантный мир, инвариантный разум. Эволюционная психология и ее критики . (2014).
- ^ Хаген, Эдвард Х. Спорные вопросы эволюционной психологии . Справочник по эволюционной психологии (2005): 145-173.
- ^ Марьянски А., Мачалек Р. и Тернер Дж. Х., 2015. Справочник по эволюции и обществу: на пути к эволюционной социальной науке. Рутледж. стр.161-163
- ^ Курзбан, Роберт (2011). Почему все (остальные) лицемеры: эволюция и модульное мышление . Нью-Джерси: Издательство Принстонского университета. ISBN 9780691146744 .
- ^ Космидес, Леда; Туби, Джон (1992). «Когнитивная адаптация для социального обмена». Адаптированный разум: эволюционная психология и порождение культуры . Нью-Йорк: Издательство Оксфордского университета. стр. 163–228.
- ^ Клюн, Джефф; Муре, Жан-Батист; Липсон, Ход (2013). «Эволюционные истоки модульности» . Труды Королевского общества . 280 (1755): 20122863.arXiv : 1207.2743 . дои : 10.1098/rspb.2012.2863 . ПМЦ 3574393 . ПМИД 23363632 .
- ^ Панксепп, Яак; Панксепп, Жюль Б. (2000). «Семь грехов эволюционной психологии» (PDF) . Эволюция и познание . 6 (2): 108–31 . Проверено 15 мая 2012 г.
- ^ Буллер, Дэвид Дж.; Хардкасл, Валери Грей (2005). «Модульность» . В Буллере, Дэвид Дж. (ред.). Адаптация разума: эволюционная психология и настойчивые поиски человеческой природы . Кембридж, Массачусетс: MIT Press . стр. 127–201 . ISBN 978-0-262-02579-9 .
- ^ Дэвис, Пол Шелдон; Фетцер, Джеймс Х.; Фостер, Томас Р. (1995). «Логические рассуждения и специфика предметной области». Биология и философия . 10 (1): 1–37. дои : 10.1007/BF00851985 . S2CID 83429932 .
- ^ О'Брайен, Дэвид; Манфринати, Анжела (2010). «Ментально-логическая теория условных суждений» . В Оксфорде, Майк; Чейтер, Ник (ред.). Познание и условные обозначения: вероятность и логика в человеческом мышлении . Нью-Йорк: Издательство Оксфордского университета. стр. 39–54. ISBN 978-0-19-923329-8 .
- ^ Космидес, Леда; Туби, Джон (13 января 1997 г.). «Эволюционная психология: Букварь» . cep.ucsb.edu . Архивировано из оригинала 24 июня 2020 года . Проверено 22 июля 2016 г.
- ^ Смит, Субрена (15 января 2020 г.). «Почему эволюционная психология (вероятно) невозможна» . Этот взгляд на жизнь . Проверено 7 февраля 2020 г.
- ^ Смит, Субрена (2020). «Возможна ли эволюционная психология?» . Биологическая теория . 15 : 39–49. дои : 10.1007/s13752-019-00336-4 . S2CID 213564464 – через Springer.
- ^ «Субрена Э. Смит, профиль факультета Университета Нью-Гэмпшира» . Профиль факультета Университета Нью-Гэмпшира . 31 мая 2018 года . Проверено 7 февраля 2021 г.
- ^ Сегерстрале, Уллика Кристина Олофсдоттер (2000). Защитники истины: битва за науку в дебатах по социобиологии и за ее пределами. Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета. ISBN 0-19-850505-1 .
- ^ Джером Х. Баркоу, (2005), Не хватает революции: дарвинизм для социологов, Оксфорд: Oxford University Press.
- ^ Баркоу, Джером (Ред.). (2006) Не хватает революции: дарвинизм для социологов. Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета. ISBN 978-0-19-513002-7
- ^ Алкок, Джон (2001). Триумф социобиологии. Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета. ISBN 978-0-19-516335-3
- ^ Сегерстрале, Уллика Кристина Олофсдоттер (2000). Защитники истины: битва за науку в дебатах по социобиологии и за ее пределами. Оксфорд [Оксфордшир]: Издательство Оксфордского университета. ISBN 0-19-850505-1 .
- ^ Туби, Дж., Космидес, Л. и Барретт, ХК (2005). Разрешение спора о врожденных идеях: ограничения обучаемости и развитое взаимопроникновение мотивационных и концептуальных функций. В книге Каррутерс П., Лоуренс С. и Стич С. (ред.), «Врожденный разум: структура и содержание». Нью-Йорк: Издательство Оксфордского университета.
- ^ Споры вокруг эволюционной психологии Эдварда Х. Хагена, Институт теоретической биологии, Берлин. В Д. М. Бассе (ред.), «Справочник по эволюционной психологии» (стр. 5–67). Хобокен, Нью-Джерси: Уайли.
- ^ Бесконечные заблуждения об эволюционной психологии: стойкие заблуждения об эволюционной психологии, Гад Саад, блог Psychology Today.
- ^ Гехер, Г. (2006). Эволюционная психология – это не зло! … и вот почему… «Психологические темы»; Специальный выпуск по эволюционной психологии, 15, 181–202. «Архивная копия» (PDF) . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 9 мая 2008 года . Проверено 9 мая 2008 г.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: архивная копия в заголовке ( ссылка ) - ^ Что на самом деле беспокоят антиэволюционные психологи , Джон Джонсон, блог Psychology Today.
- ^ Курзбан, Р. (2013). Это относится к «Одиннадцати», П. З. Майерсу и другим изюминкам. Эволюционная психология.
- ^ «Семь ключевых заблуждений об эволюционной психологии» . Арео . 20 августа 2019 г. Проверено 23 декабря 2022 г.
Ссылки
[ редактировать ]- Баркоу, Джером Х. (2006). Не хватает революции: дарвинизм для социологов . Издательство Оксфордского университета, США. ISBN 978-0-19-513002-7 .
- Баркоу, Дж.; Космидес, Л.; Туби, Дж. (1992). Адаптированный разум: Эволюционная психология и порождение культуры . Оксфорд: Издательство Оксфордского университета.
- Боулби, Джон (1969). Вложение . Нью-Йорк: Основные книги. ISBN 9780465097159 .
- Басс, ДМ; Барнс, М. (1986). «Предпочтения в выборе партнера для человека» (PDF) . Журнал личности и социальной психологии . 50 (3): 559–70. дои : 10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.559 .
- Басс, DM (1988). «От бдительности к насилию: Тактика удержания партнера у американских студентов» (PDF) . Этология и социобиология . 9 (5): 291–317. дои : 10.1016/0162-3095(88)90010-6 . hdl : 2027.42/27156 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 20 марта 2009 года . Проверено 16 июля 2008 г.
- Басс, DM (1989). «Половые различия в предпочтениях человека к партнеру: эволюционные гипотезы, проверенные в 37 культурах» (PDF) . Поведенческие и мозговые науки . 12 :1–49. дои : 10.1017/S0140525X00023992 .
- Басс, ДМ; Ларсен, Р.Дж.; Вестен, Д.; Семмельрот, Дж. (1992). «Половые различия в ревности: эволюция, физиология и психология» (PDF) . Психологическая наука . 3 (4): 251–55. дои : 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00038.x . S2CID 27388562 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 1 июня 2013 года . Проверено 16 июля 2008 г.
- Басс, DM (1994). Эволюция желания: стратегии спаривания человека . Нью-Йорк: Основные книги.
- Басс, Дэвид М. (2004). Эволюционная психология: новая наука о разуме . Бостон: Пирсон / A и B. ISBN 978-0-205-37071-9 .
- Басс, Дэвид М.; Хазелтон, Марти Г.; Шекелфорд, Тодд К.; Блеске, Эйприл Л.; Уэйкфилд, Джером К. (1998). «Адаптации, экзаптации и спандрелы» . Американский психолог . 53 (5): 533–48. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.387.5882 . дои : 10.1037/0003-066X.53.5.533 . ПМИД 9612136 . S2CID 11128780 . Архивировано из оригинала 21 октября 2002 года . Проверено 29 августа 2011 г.
- Кларк, Мюррей (2004). Реконструкция разума и представления . Кембридж, Массачусетс: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-03322-0 .
- Конфер, Истон; Истон, Джудит А.; Флейшман, Диана С.; Гетц, Кэри Д.; Льюис, Дэвид М.Г.; Периллу, Карин; Басс, Дэвид М. (2010). «Эволюционная психология» (PDF) . Американский психолог . 65 (2): 110–126. дои : 10.1037/a0018413 . ПМИД 20141266 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 20 августа 2015 года.
- Дантли, доктор медицинских наук; Басс, DM (2008). «Эволюционная психология — это метатеория психологии» (PDF) . Психологический опрос . 19 :30–34. дои : 10.1080/10478400701774105 . S2CID 12267555 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 20 мая 2014 года . Проверено 14 февраля 2011 г.
- Даррант, Р.; Эллис, Би Джей (2003). «Эволюционная психология». В Галлахере, М.; Нельсон, Р.Дж. (ред.). Комплексный справочник по психологии, том третий: биологическая психология . Нью-Йорк: Wiley & Sons. стр. 1–33.
- Эван, Дилан (2000). Введение в эволюционную психологию . Лэнхэм, Мэриленд: Totem Books USA. ISBN 978-1-84046-043-8 .
- Гаулин, Стивен Дж. К. и Дональд Х. Макберни. Эволюционная психология . Прентис Холл. 2003. ISBN 978-0-13-111529-3
- Хант, Линн (2014). «Я и его история» . Американский исторический обзор . 119 (5): 1576–86. дои : 10.1093/ahr/119.5.1576 .
- Джойс, Ричард (2006). Эволюция морали (Жизнь и разум: философские проблемы биологии и психологии) . Кембридж, Массачусетс: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-10112-7 .
- Миллер, Джеффри П. (2000). Брачный разум: как сексуальный выбор повлиял на эволюцию человеческой природы . Гарден-Сити, Нью-Йорк: Даблдей. ISBN 978-0-385-49516-5 .
- Нарваес, Д; Ван, Л; Глисон, Т; Ченг, Ю; Лефевер, Дж; Дэн, Л. (2012). «Развитая ниша развития и социоморальные результаты китайских трехлетних детей». Европейский журнал психологии развития . 10 (2): 106–127. дои : 10.1080/17405629.2012.761606 . S2CID 143327355 .
- Нарваес, Д; Глисон, Т; Ван, Л; Брукс, Дж; Лефевер, Дж; Ченг, Ю (2013). «Ниша развитого развития: продольное влияние практики ухода на психосоциальное развитие детей раннего возраста». Ежеквартальный журнал исследований раннего детства . 28 (4): 759–773. дои : 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.003 .
- Нессе, РМ (2000). Четыре вопроса Тингергена. Архивировано 18 января 2012 года в Wayback Machine .
- Нессе, Р ; Уильямс, Джордж К. (1996). Почему мы болеем . Нью-Йорк: Винтаж.
- Пинкер, Стивен (1997). Как работает разум . Нью-Йорк: Нортон. ISBN 978-0-393-04535-2 .
- Пинкер, Стивен (2002). Чистый лист: современное отрицание человеческой природы . Нью-Йорк, штат Нью-Йорк: Викинг. ISBN 978-0-670-03151-1 .
- Ричардс, Джанет С. (2000). Человеческая природа после Дарвина: философское введение . Нью-Йорк: Рутледж. ISBN 978-0-415-21243-4 .
- Райан, Кристофер ; Джета, Касильда (2010). Секс на рассвете: доисторические истоки современной сексуальности . Нью-Йорк, штат Нью-Йорк: Харпер . ISBN 9780062002938 . OCLC 668224740 .
- Сантрок, Джон В. (2005). Актуальный подход к развитию продолжительности жизни (3-е изд.) . Нью-Йорк, штат Нью-Йорк: МакГроу Хилл. ISBN 978-0-07-322626-2 .
- Шактер, Дэниел Л., Дэниел Вегнер и Дэниел Гилберт. 2007. Психология . Стоит издательства. ISBN 0-7167-5215-8 ISBN 9780716752158 .
- Шмитт, ДП; Басс, DM (2001). «Браконьерство среди людей: тактика и искушения для проникновения в существующие отношения» (PDF) . Журнал личности и социальной психологии . 80 (6): 894–917. дои : 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.894 . ПМИД 11414373 .
- Туби, Дж.; Космидес, Л. (2005). «Концептуальные основы эволюционной психологии». В Бассе, Д.М. (ред.). Справочник по эволюционной психологии (PDF) . Хобокен, Нью-Джерси: Уайли. стр. 5–67. Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 17 декабря 2018 года.
- Уилсон, Эдвард Осборн («ЭО») (1975). Социобиология: новый синтез . Кембридж: Belknap Press издательства Гарвардского университета. ISBN 978-0674816213 .
- Райт, Роберт CM (1995). Моральное животное: эволюционная психология и повседневная жизнь . Нью-Йорк: Винтажные книги. ISBN 978-0-679-76399-4 .
Дальнейшее чтение
[ редактировать ]- Басс, DM (1995). «Эволюционная психология: новая парадигма психологической науки» (PDF) . Психологический опрос . 6 :1–30. дои : 10.1207/s15327965pli0601_1 . Архивировано из оригинала (PDF) 12 сентября 2015 года . Проверено 20 мая 2007 г.
- Конфер, JC; Истон, Дж.А.; Флейшман, Д.С.; Гетц, CD; Льюис, DMG; Периллу, К.; Басс, DM (2010). «Эволюционная психология: противоречия, вопросы, перспективы и ограничения» (PDF) . Американский психолог . 65 (2): 110–26. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.601.8691 . дои : 10.1037/a0018413 . ПМИД 20141266 .
- Космидес, Леда ; Туби, Джон (2008). «Эволюционная психология» . В Хамови, Рональд (ред.). Эволюционная психология . Энциклопедия либертарианства . Таузенд-Оукс, Калифорния: SAGE ; Институт Катона . стр. 158–61. дои : 10.4135/9781412965811.n99 . ISBN 978-1-4129-6580-4 . LCCN 2008009151 . OCLC 750831024 .
- Хейлиген Ф. (2012). « Эволюционная психология », в: А. Михалос (ред.): Энциклопедия исследований качества жизни (Springer, Берлин).
- Кеннэр, Лео (2002). «Эволюционная психология: новая интегративная перспектива в науке и практике психологии» . Обзор человеческой природы . 2 : 17–61.
- Медикус, Г. (2005). «Эволюционная теория гуманитарных наук» . стр. 9, 10, 11 . Проверено 8 сентября 2009 г.
- Герхард Медикус (2017). Быть человеком: преодоление разрыва между науками о теле и разуме, Берлинский VWB
- Ойкконен, Венла: гендер, сексуальность и воспроизводство в эволюционных повествованиях. Лондон: Рутледж, 2013. ISBN 978-0-415-63599-8
Внешние ссылки
[ редактировать ]- PsychTable.org Совместная работа по каталогизации психологических адаптаций человека.
- Эволюционная психология в Керли
- Что такое эволюционная психология? Клинический эволюционный психолог Дейл Глебах .
- Эволюционная психология - Подходы в психологии
- Герхард Медикус (2017). Быть человеком: преодоление разрыва между науками о теле и разуме, Берлинский VWB
Академические общества
[ редактировать ]- Общество человеческого поведения и эволюции ; международное общество, занимающееся использованием эволюционной теории для изучения человеческой природы
- Международное общество этологии человека ; продвигает этологические взгляды на изучение человека во всем мире
- Европейская ассоциация человеческого поведения и эволюции - междисциплинарное общество, которое поддерживает деятельность европейских исследователей, интересующихся эволюционными теориями человеческого познания, поведения и общества.
- Ассоциация политики и наук о жизни ; международная междисциплинарная ассоциация ученых, ученых и политиков, занимающихся эволюционными, генетическими и экологическими знаниями и их влиянием на политическое поведение, государственную политику и этику.
- Общество эволюционного анализа в праве - научная ассоциация, занимающаяся содействием междисциплинарному исследованию проблем на стыке права, биологии и теории эволюции.
- Институт когнитивных наук и эволюционной психологии Новой Англии стремится способствовать исследованиям и образованию в области междисциплинарной связи когнитивной науки и эволюционных исследований.
- Северо-восточное общество эволюционной психологии ; региональное общество, занимающееся поощрением научных исследований и диалога по теме эволюционной психологии
- Исследователи Общества феминистской эволюционной психологии , изучающие активную роль женщин в эволюции человека.
Журналы
[ редактировать ]- Эволюционная психология - научный онлайн-журнал в бесплатном доступе
- Эволюция и поведение человека - журнал Общества человеческого поведения и эволюции
- Эволюционная психологическая наука — международный междисциплинарный форум оригинальных исследовательских работ, посвященных эволюционной психологии. Охватывает социальные и биологические науки, антропологию, философию, криминологию, право и гуманитарные науки.
- Политика и науки о жизни - междисциплинарный рецензируемый журнал, издаваемый Ассоциацией политики и наук о жизни.
- Человеческая природа: междисциплинарная биосоциальная перспектива - продвигает междисциплинарные исследования биологических, социальных и экологических факторов, которые лежат в основе человеческого поведения. Основное внимание уделяется, прежде всего, функциональному единству, в котором эти факторы непрерывно и взаимно взаимодействуют. К ним относятся эволюционные, биологические и социологические процессы, взаимодействующие с социальным поведением человека.
- Биологическая теория: интеграция развития, эволюции и познания - посвящена теоретическим достижениям в области биологии и познания с упором на концептуальную интеграцию, обеспечиваемую эволюционными подходами и подходами к развитию.
- Эволюционная антропология
- Поведенческие науки и науки о мозге — междисциплинарные статьи по психологии, нейробиологии, поведенческой биологии, когнитивной науке, искусственному интеллекту, лингвистике и философии. Около 30% статей посвящены эволюционному анализу поведения.
- Эволюция и развитие - исследования, имеющие отношение к стыку эволюционной биологии и биологии развития.
- Эволюционный обзор – искусство, наука и культура
Видео
[ редактировать ]- Краткий видеоролик из сериала PBS «Эволюция».
- Выступление на TED. Архивировано 22 октября 2011 года в Wayback Machine Стивена Пинкера о его книге «Чистый лист: современное отрицание человеческой природы».
- Выступление эволюционного психолога Роберта Курцбана в RSA о модульности мышления, основанное на его книге « Почему все (остальные) — лицемеры»
- Лекция Ричарда Докинза о естественном отборе и эволюционной психологии
- Эволюционная психология - Стивен Пинкер и Франс де Ваал Аудиозапись
- Разум каменного века: беседа с эволюционными психологами Ледой Космидес и Джоном Туби
- Маргарет Мид и Самоа . Обзор дебатов о природе и воспитании, вызванных книгой Мида «Достижение совершеннолетия в Самоа».
- «Эволюционная психология» , «В наше время », дискуссия на BBC Radio 4 с Джанет Рэдклифф Ричардс, Николасом Хамфри и Стивеном Роузом (2 ноября 2000 г.)